Skip to main content

GENERAL COMMENTARY article

Front. Psychol., 26 July 2017
Sec. Cognitive Science

Commentary: Alignment in social interactions

  • 1Departamento de Ciencias de la Computación, Instituto de Investigaciones en Matemáticas Aplicadas y en Sistemas, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Mexico City, Mexico
  • 2Centro de Ciencias de la Complejidad, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Mexico City, Mexico
  • 3Plan de Estudios Combinados en Medicina (MD/PhD), Facultad de Medicina, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México, Mexico City, Mexico

A Commentary on
Alignment in social interactions

By Gallotti, M., Fairhurst, M. T., and Frith, C. D. (2017). Conscious. Cogn. 48, 253–261. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.12.002

We welcome Gallotti et al. 's (2017) proposal to shift the study of social cognition from focusing on types of representation to types of interaction. This aligns with the enactive approach to social cognition (e.g., Froese and Di Paolo, 2011), which has long been arguing for this kind of shift (e.g., Varela, 2000; De Jaegher and Di Paolo, 2007). We offer some clarifications from this latter perspective, which will hopefully benefit the development of their proposal.

Gallotti et al. point to “reciprocal exchange of information” (p. 255) as the key criterion of social interaction. An immediate issue is that the term “information” is ambiguous. They mostly use it in a technical sense (e.g., following Friston and Frith's (2015) account of generalized synchrony), but there are semantic connotations (e.g., “systems reciprocate thoughts and experiences”, p. 255), an equivocation that is problematic (Hutto and Myin, 2013). Here we stick to the technical sense.

An example of reciprocity leading to mutual alignment are members of an audience clapping: hearing each other influences their clapping, thereby facilitating moments of spontaneous synchrony. In general, entrainment of coupled systems can indeed be explained by reciprocal information exchange, like two grandfather clocks synchronizing their pendulums via sonic influence (Oliveira and Melo, 2015). Thus, Gallotti et al. have rediscovered an important insight from the cybernetics era: there is a key difference between coupling two non-linear systems in a unidirectional (feedforward) and bidirectional (feedback) circuit, with the latter interaction giving rise to novel properties at the level of the integrated system.

More recently, this insight has been applied to the study of social interaction by several research programs, including in neuroscience (Schilbach et al., 2013), evolutionary robotics (Froese et al., 2013), and especially in psychology (Riley et al., 2011; Fusaroli and Tylén, 2015). They confirm that social interactions can only be fully understood at the collective level.

Gallotti et al. acknowledge some of these traditions, but surprisingly they do not refer to the perceptual crossing paradigm (Auvray and Rohde, 2012), an experimental setup designed for studying real-time interaction (see Figure 1A for an explanation) that can give rise to their five types of alignment (see their Figures 1–5). Especially their on-line types of alignment have been studied systematically, as exemplified by Figure 1B based on data taken from (Froese et al., 2014).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Pairs of physically separated participants are embodied as avatars in an invisible virtual reality (VR) environment consisting of a line that wraps around. They can move their avatar and their hand will receive a tactile stimulation when it overlaps with another object. Each player can encounter a static object and two mobile objects: the other's avatar and a “shadow” that is copying the other's movements. The task for the players is to locate the other's avatar (and to mark this event by clicking). (B) Type 3: “on-line social cognition without mutual alignment” happens when A is moving and B is following A's shadow such that B's movements align with those of A but not vice versa (highlighted in gray). Type 4: “on-line social cognition with mutual alignment” happens when the avatars respond to each other's contact, e.g. by oscillating back and forth (highlighted in purple). Such perceptual crossing satisfies Gallotti et al.'s criterion for social interaction as reciprocal information exchange. A and B reported clear and almost clear awareness of the other, respectively, at the time of their click. (C) Type 5: type 4 situations in which it happens that one participant adapts more than another. Here A initially adapts more than B by imitating B's oscillatory movements (highlighted in pink). But the interaction then quickly gives rise to turn-taking and mutual imitation, which go beyond mere reciprocal dynamics because they depend on active co-regulation. Both participants reported clear awareness of the other at the time of their click.

In line with Gallotti et al.'s claim that the “pooling together of individual mental resources generates results that exceed the sum of the individual contributions” (p. 259), these experiments have repeatedly shown that players most frequently judge to be interacting socially precisely during mutual interaction (i.e., most clicks happen during type 4 and 5 situations). However, at the same time there is no statistically significant recognition of the other player, as measured in terms of individuals' sensitivity to social contingency for a particular virtual encounter (i.e., a player is equally likely to click following an encounter with the other's “avatar” and with the other's nonresponsive “shadow”). So instead this overall objective success has been explained at the collective level in terms of differences in the relative stability of the types of interactions (e.g., Di Paolo et al., 2008). This counterintuitive finding supports Gallotti et al.'s assertion that reciprocity can lead to mutual alignment, but is not sufficient for shared awareness (p. 256).

However, is this a case of online social cognition? Some have argued that it is (De Jaegher et al., 2010), but others have raised a number of concerns (e.g., Overgaard and Michael, 2015), which would similarly apply to Gallotti et al.'s proposal. Specifically, it is unclear whether an interactional factor that remains independent from an individual's judgments (or of their awareness of the other's presence) should count as cognitive (or social).

Froese and Di Paolo (2011) proposed the more conservative notion of a “multi-agent system” for such situations. They also argued that two additional criteria are required to go from dynamical reciprocity to social interaction: (1) they appeal to normativity to differentiate actions from physical happenings (thereby introducing a dependence on intentionality), and (2) they appeal to co-regulation to differentiate individually realizable actions from those requiring another responsive agent (thereby introducing a dependence on sociality). They also hypothesized that interactions satisfying these two criteria will give rise to social awareness.

The first criterion can be satisfied by reciprocal interactions in the perceptual crossing paradigm, but the second criterion may often remain unsatisfied, thus accounting for the lack of individual social sensitivity. Froese et al. (2014) investigated what happens when participants are encouraged to act collaboratively. They found that turn-taking emerged as a paradigmatic example of co-regulation, and that it was associated with clear social awareness shared by both participants (see Figure 1C). Other examples include mutual imitation and negotiating leader and follower roles.

Further analyses of the time series of participants' movements by Zapata-Fonseca et al. (2016) revealed they are characterized by complexity matching, which means that the clustering statistics of salient events followed a power-law distribution and that the scaling was similar across paired participants. They were of a similar form as has been found to support multi-scalar mutual alignment during vocal interactions (Abney et al., 2014). Yet complexity matching was not significantly associated with objectively more accurate social recognition or subjectively clearer social awareness. In other words, while mutual alignment can be understood as a relatively spontaneous property of a multi-agent sensorimotor loop, in which sensory information and motor activity are mutually adapting, it is not a sufficient marker of on-line social cognition per se. As suggested by Garrod and Pickering (2004), such lower-level automatic alignments are the substrate for higher-level dyadic properties to emerge.

In sum, reciprocal information exchange and mutual alignment are important aspects of bidirectional interactions, but Gallotti et al. require more conceptual resources to be able to explain how people “take one another into account” as other individuals. For the enactive approach the challenge is to account for the missing factors without falling back on appeals to internal mental representations and theory of mind (Hutto, 2008).

Author Contributions

TF wrote the initial draft of the manuscript, LZ added further comments and produced the figure. Both TF and LZ finalized the manuscript together.

Funding

This work was supported by UNAM-DGAPA-PAPIIT project “Explorando los alcances de la auto-organización social: Desde la cultura hasta la célula” (IA104717), and by CONACyT (grant CB2013-221341). LZ acknowledges additional support from CONACyT (grant CB-2011-01_167441) and PAPIIT (grant IV100116).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Abney, D. H., Paxton, A., Dale, R., and Kello, C. T. (2014). Complexity matching in dyadic conversation. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 143, 2304–2315. doi: 10.1037/xge0000021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Auvray, M., and Rohde, M. (2012). Perceptual crossing: the simplest online paradigm. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:181. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Jaegher, H., and Di Paolo, E. A. (2007). Participatory sense-making: an enactive approach to social cognition. Phenomenol. Cogn. Sci. 6, 485–507. doi: 10.1007/s11097-007-9076-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Jaegher, H., Di Paolo, E. A., and Gallagher, S. (2010). Can social interaction constitute social cognition? Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 441–447. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Paolo, E. A., Rohde, M., and Iizuka, H. (2008). Sensitivity to social contingency or stability of interaction? Modelling the dynamics of perceptual crossing. New Ideas Psychol. 26, 278–294. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2007.07.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. J., and Frith, C. D. (2015). A duet for one. Conscious. Cogn. 36, 390–405. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Froese, T., and Di Paolo, E. A. (2011). The enactive approach: theoretical sketches from cell to society. Pragmat. Cogn. 19, 1–36. doi: 10.1075/pc.19.1.01fro

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Froese, T., Iizuka, H., and Ikegami, T. (2013). From synthetic modeling of social interaction to dynamic theories of brain-body-environment-body-brain systems. Behav. Brain Sci. 36, 420–421. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12001902

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Froese, T., Iizuka, H., and Ikegami, T. (2014). Embodied social interaction constitutes social cognition in pairs of humans: a minimalist virtual reality experiment. Sci. Rep. 4:3672. doi: 10.1038/srep03672

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fusaroli, R., and Tylén, K. (2015). Investigating conversational dynamics: interactive alignment, interpersonal synergy, and collective task performance. Cogn. Sci. 40, 145–171. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gallotti, M., Fairhurst, M. T., and Frith, C. D. (2017). Alignment in social interactions. Conscious. Cogn. 48, 253–261. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2016.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garrod, S., and Pickering, M. J. (2004). Why is conversation so easy? Trends Cogn. Sci. 8, 8–11. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.10.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutto, D. D. (2008). Folk Psychological Narratives: The Sociocultural Basis of Understanding Reasons. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Hutto, D. D., and Myin, E. (2013). Radicalizing Enactivism: Basic Minds without Content. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

Oliveira, H. M., and Melo, L. V. (2015). Huygens synchronization of two clocks. Sci. Rep. 5:11548. doi: 10.1038/srep11548

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Overgaard, S., and Michael, J. (2015). The interactive turn in social cognition research: a critique. Philos. Psychol. 28, 160–183. doi: 10.1080/09515089.2013.827109

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riley, M. A., Richardson, M. J., Shockley, K., and Ramenzoni, V. C. (2011). Interpersonal synergies. Front. Psychol. 2:38. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Varela, F. J. (2000). “Steps to a science of inter-being: Unfolding the Dharma implicit in modern cognitive science,” in The Psychology of Awakening: Buddhism, Science, and Our Day-to-Day Lives, eds G. Watson, S. Batchelor, and G. Claxton (York Beach, ME: Samuel Weiser, Inc), 71–89.

Google Scholar

Schilbach, L., Timmermans, B., Reddy, V., Costall, A., Bente, G., Schlicht, T., et al. (2013). Toward a second-person neuroscience. Behav. Brain Sci. 36, 393–462. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X12000660

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zapata-Fonseca, L., Dotov, D., Fossion, R., and Froese, T. (2016). Time-series analysis of embodied interaction: movement variability and complexity matching as dyadic properties. Front. Psychol. 7:1940. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01940

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: social cognition, social interaction, interpersonal coordination, embodied cognition, enactive cognition

Citation: Froese T and Zapata-Fonseca L (2017) Commentary: Alignment in social interactions. Front. Psychol. 8:1249. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01249

Received: 14 March 2017; Accepted: 10 July 2017;
Published: 26 July 2017.

Edited by:

Serge Thill, Plymouth University, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Alessandra Sciutti, Fondazione Istituto Italiano di Technologia, Italy

Copyright © 2017 Froese and Zapata-Fonseca. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Tom Froese, t.froese@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.