Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 22 August 2017
Sec. Decision Neuroscience

Modulating the Activity of the DLPFC and OFC Has Distinct Effects on Risk and Ambiguity Decision-Making: A tDCS Study

  • 1School of Business and Management, Shanghai International Studies University, Shanghai, China
  • 2Academy of Financial Research, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China
  • 3College of Economics, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China
  • 4Neuro and Behavior EconLab, Zhejiang University of Finance and Economics, Hangzhou, China
  • 5Interdisciplinary Center for Social Sciences, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China

Human beings are constantly exposed to two types of uncertainty situations, risk and ambiguity. Neuroscientific studies suggest that the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and the orbital frontal cortex (OFC) play significant roles in human decision making under uncertainty. We applied the transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) device to modulate the activity of participants’ DLPFC and OFC separately, comparing the causal relationships between people’s behaviors and the activity of the corresponding brain cortex when confronted with situations of risk and ambiguity. Our experiment employed a pre–post design and a risk/ambiguity decision-making task, from which we could calculate the preferences via an estimation model. We found evidences that modulating the activity of the DLPFC using right anodal/left cathodal tDCS significantly enhanced the participants’ preferences for risk, whereas modulating the activity of the OFC with right anodal/left cathodal tDCS significantly decreased the participants’ preferences for ambiguity. The reverse effects were also observed in the reversed tDCS treatments on the two areas. Our results suggest that decision-making processes under risk and ambiguity are complicated and may be encoded in two distinct circuits in our brains as the DLPFC primarily impacts decisions under risk whereas the OFC affects ambiguity.

Introduction

Human beings are constantly exposed to uncertainty situations in which the careful weighing of possible outcomes is required to make decisions. In certain situations, the probabilities of possible outcomes can be determined, such as the gamble of coin tossing, whereas more generically, the probabilities are unknown, such as the chance of a sunny day. These two types of uncertainty situations are, respectively, called risk and ambiguity (Ellsberg, 1961; Curley and Yates, 1985). The behavioral differences of people facing risk and ambiguity have been demonstrated in various experimental economic studies (Ellsberg, 1961; Camerer and Weber, 1992). Individuals who are required to make decisions rely deeply on information regarding the probabilities of any possible consequences (Frisch and Baron, 1988; Fox and Tversky, 1995). Several studies suggest that individuals are more willing to bet on risky outcomes than on ambiguous ones for the same benefits (Ellsberg, 1961; Camerer and Weber, 1992). Explanations for this ambiguity aversion have included informed opponents (Kuhberger and Perner, 2003; Hsu et al., 2005) and comparative ignorance (Fox and Tversky, 1995). However, because the majority of experimental studies have focused on the risk situation and have seldom considered ambiguity, the relationship between risk and ambiguity behavior is still far from settled.

In fact, people’s attitudes toward risk and ambiguity may be distinct constructs. One recent study indicates that changes in ambiguity attitudes are age-related but those in risk attitudes are not (Blankenstein et al., 2016). Recent neuroscientific studies also find that the decision-making processes under risk and ambiguity are separately encoded in our brains (Hsu et al., 2005; Knoch et al., 2006). Neuroimaging studies have indicated the significant roles of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and the orbital frontal cortex (OFC) in human decision making under uncertainty (Hsu et al., 2005; Kuhnen and Knutson, 2005; Huettel et al., 2006; Krain et al., 2006; Lin et al., 2014). Nevertheless, the results of these studies are various. Some studies find that the DLPFC is activated during risk decisions, whereas the OFC is more active in the situation of ambiguity than in the situation of risk (Bechara et al., 1996). PFC-lesioned participants tend to make riskier decisions, whereas OFC-lesioned participants are not sensitive to the degree of ambiguity and risk (Hsu et al., 2005). On the contrary, the meta-analysis by Krain et al. (2006) indicates that ambiguous decision-making is associated with activity in DLPFC while risky decision-making is associated with activity in OFC.

Because neuroimaging studies can hardly demonstrate a causal relationship between the activity of certain cortex areas and the specific types of preference, brain stimulation techniques are needed to investigate how modulating the cortex activity of the DLPFC and OFC will differently affect participants’ preferences for risk and ambiguity. Such studies find evidence that modulating activity in the DLPFC using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) can change people’s decisions in uncertainty situations. However, the results of these studies also remain controversial. Certain studies find a positive relationship between the activity level of the right DLPFC and risk-taking behaviors (Ye et al., 2015a,b, 2016; Huang et al., 2017), while others find a negative or insignificant relationship (Knoch et al., 2006; Fecteau et al., 2007a,b). These varying results may be attributed to the confusion of the risk and ambiguity tasks applied in the studies, such as the Rogers’ Risk Task (Rogers et al., 1999), the Balloon Analogue Risk Task and the risk measurement table (Ye et al., 2015b). In addition, there is a lack of research investigating the effect of modulating activity in the OFC on participants’ preferences of risk and ambiguity.

In this study, we used a tDCS device to separately modulate the activity of participants’ DLPFC and OFC, comparing the causal relationships between people’s behaviors and the activity of the corresponding brain cortex when confronted with situations of risk and ambiguity. Our experiment employed a pre–post design and a risk/ambiguity decision-making task, from which we could separate the participants’ preferences for risk and ambiguity and calculate the preferences via an estimation model derived from Hsu et al. (2005). By comparing the participants’ preferences before and after the stimulation, we were able to identify whether the various stimulations had changed the participants’ risk and ambiguity attitudes. We aimed to find distinct effects of different stimulation treatments on different behaviors of risk and ambiguity.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A total of 72 students from Zhejiang University (42 males, 30 females; mean age 21.94 years, ranging from 18 to 28 years) participated in our experiment. Recruited by the campus BBS, the students had diverse majors including Arts, Science, Social Science, Engineering, Agriculture, Medicine, etc. All participants were right-handed and declared no history of psychiatric illness or neurological disorders, with no experience of tDCS or risk/ambiguity decision-making tasks. To participate in the experiment, they were required to provide written informed consent approved by the Zhejiang University ethics committee. The participants were randomly assigned to three stimulation montages (right anodal/left cathodal, left anodal/right cathodal and sham stimulation) with two types of target area (DLPFC and OFC); thus, there were six different treatments with 12 participants in each treatment. The experiment lasted approximately 1 h, and each participant received an average of 55.29 RMB yuan (approximately 7.95 United States dollars) for their participation. Although several participants reported slight itches during the experiment, no adverse side effects regarding pain on the scalp or headaches were reported after the experiment.

Transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS)

Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) was delivered by a battery-driven stimulator (multichannel non-invasive wireless tDCS neurostimulator, Starlab, Barcelona, Spain) via two saline-soaked surface sponge electrodes (35 cm2) fixed on the scalp of the participant with a rubber belt. The current had a constant intensity of 2 mA, delivered for 20 min with 30 s of ramp up and down. This montage would induce cortical excitability change in the target area without causing any physiological damage to the participants. The anodal electrode would enhance cortical excitability while the cathodal electrode would inhibit it (Nitsche and Paulus, 2000). For the sham stimulation, the current was delivered only for 30 s once it reached 2 mA. However, the participants treated this as the regular process of the stimulation and were unaware of their stimulation types, according to the questionnaire after the experiment. This method of sham stimulation has also been shown to be reliable by previous literature (Gandiga et al., 2006).

The target areas were localized according to the International 10–20 System. For DLPFC stimulation, the anodal (cathodal) electrode was placed over the right F4 and the cathodal (anodal) electrode was placed over the left F3 in the right anodal/left cathodal (left anodal/right cathodal) treatment (Figure 1A). For OFC stimulation, the anodal (cathodal) electrode was placed over the right Fp2 and the cathodal (anodal) electrode was placed over the left Fp1 in the right anodal/left cathodal (left anodal/right cathodal) treatment (Figure 1B). These stimulation montages were proved effective in modulating the activity of DLPFC and OFC, respectively, by previous literatures (Fecteau et al., 2007a,b; Merzagora et al., 2010; Ouellet et al., 2015; Willis et al., 2015; Wang et al., 2016). Furthermore, Figure 2 demonstrates the electric simulations of DLPFC and OFC stimulations performed with the tDCS neurostimulator software. We can see that the voltage distributions of the cortex were quite different between the two stimulation treatments. Electrodes placed over F4 and F3 mainly affected the DLPFC area while electrodes placed over Fp2 and Fp1 mainly affected the OFC area.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Electrode placements in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) stimulations (A) and orbital frontal cortex (OFC) stimulations (B).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Electric simulations performed with the tDCS neurostimulator software. (A,B) simulations of right anodal/left cathodal DLPFC stimulation; (C,D) simulations of right anodal/left cathodal OFC stimulation.

Experiment Design

Risk/Ambiguity Decision-Making Task

The risk/ambiguity decision-making task is modified from Hsu et al. (2005), aiming to distinguish the participants’ preferences for pure risk and entire ambiguity via simple either-or choices. The task is composed of 48 choices; in each choice, the participants choose between a constant payoff (denoted as C.P.) and participation in a gamble. In the gamble, the computer will randomly select a ball from a given number of balls. There are only two types of balls, red and blue. In half of the choices, the composition of the balls is available to the participants, and thus, these choices will degenerate into pure risk decision-making (denoted as risk choices). In the rest of the choices, the composition of the balls is randomly decided by the computer and is unavailable to the participants, and thus, these choices will trigger ambiguity decision-making (denoted as ambiguity choices).

If the participant chooses the C.P., he (she) will obtain the exact amount of money. If he (she) chooses to take the gamble, then he (she) will obtain a higher gambling payoff (G.P.) when the red ball is selected but will obtain nothing if the blue ball is selected. The parameters of each choice are displayed in Table 1. For instance, if the participant chooses to take the gamble in Choice 1, the computer will randomly select a ball from 3 red balls and 1 blue ball. The participant will obtain 30 RMB yuan if it is red or nothing otherwise. If the participant chooses to take the gamble in Choice 48, the computer will first randomly decide the composition of the 40 balls and then select a random ball from them. The participant will obtain 25 RMB yuan or nothing depending on the result.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. The risk/ambiguity decision-making task.

Procedure

The experiment began with the risk/ambiguity decision-making task run by the experimental software z-Tree (Fischbacher, 2007). The 48 choices were mixed and presented randomly one by one. After the task, the experimenter placed a tDCS device on the participant’s head for stimulation and told him (her) to calmly rest for 20 min. The tDCS device was stopped and taken away when the stimulation ended.

Next, the risk/ambiguity decision-making task was administered for a second time with different choice orders. However, the participants were not informed and demonstrated ignorance of the two tasks’ identity according to the questionnaire. All participants were asked to complete the questionnaire concerning personal information and experiment-related feelings before receiving their payments.

When the participants finished the questionnaire, the computer randomly selected one choice from the 48 choices in each task. The payments of the participants were the combination of a show-up fee and the payoffs in the selected choices according to their decisions and the randomness of the computer selections.

Data Analysis

The participants’ selections in the risk/ambiguity decision-making task indicated their preferences for risk and ambiguity, from which we could determine certain depicting parameters. The model for parameter estimation is derived from Hsu et al. (2005). It assumes that participants have the following expected utility function: U(p, γ, x, ρ) = π(p, γ)u(x, ρ), in which u(x, ρ) is the possible utility and π(p, γ) is the subjective probability. This type of linear utility function is widely used in economics (Savage, 1954/1972; Hsu et al., 2005; Bouyssou and Marchant, 2011). u(x, ρ) =xρ, where x is the possible payoff while ρ ≥ 0 is interpreted as the preference for pure risk: ρ > 1 indicates risk preferring, ρ < 1 indicates risk aversion, and ρ = 1 indicates risk neutral (Huettel et al., 2006). π(p, γ) =pγ, where p is the probability for the payoff while γ ≥ 0 is interpreted as the degree of ambiguity aversion: γ > 1 indicates ambiguity aversion, γ < 1 indicates ambiguity preferring, and γ = 1 indicates ambiguity neutral.

For C.P. options, p = 1. For gamble options in risky choices, p is the proportion of red balls in the given number of balls (i.e., the prob. in Table 1), and γ = 1 is constrained because the probabilities are given with no ambiguity. For gamble options in ambiguity choices, p is assumed to be 0.5 because the probabilities are randomly decided by the computer, while γ is left for estimation. In each choice, if the utility of the C.P. option is higher than the gamble option, the participant is predicted to select the C.P. option; otherwise, he/she is predicted to select the gamble option. We applied the Nelder–Mead simplex algorithm to find the value of ρ and γ that maximized the number of correct predictions (Huettel et al., 2006). Because the parameter estimates can be highly uncertain in one estimation, we repeated the estimations for 100 times and chose the one with the maximum number of correct predictions as the estimated value of ρ and γ. To test the robustness of our estimated values with respect to the participant’s selections, we also drew 1000 bootstrap samples and calculated the bootstrap errors of the estimated values. The estimations were performed using MATLAB software (MathWorks, Natick, MA, United States).

We used multiple types of ANOVAs to analyze the behavioral data and the estimated parameter values of the participants. We successively examined the number of C.P. selections, the parameter values of the participants and the effect of stimulation on different parameters in different treatments. We also examined the gender difference of the participants in our experiment. Multiple comparisons were adjusted by Bonferroni correction. The critical level of significance was set at p < 0.05. All statistical tests were performed using SPSS20 (SPSSInc., Chicago, IL, United States).

Results

C.P. Selections

We first examine the behavioral data of the participants. In each risk/ambiguity choice, the participants should choose between the C.P. and the gamble. More C.P. selections can roughly indicate a stronger tendency of risk/ambiguity aversion before we obtain the estimated parameter values. Figure 3 displays the average number of C.P. selections in different choices and treatments before and after the stimulation. We performed one-way ANOVA using area (DLPFC vs. OFC) and stimulation type (right anodal/left cathodal, left anodal/right cathodal or sham) as the factor, respectively, while the number of C.P. selections served as the dependent variable. No significant effect of area or stimulation type was indicated on the number of both risk and ambiguity C.P. selections before the stimulations. In this sense, the participants were well mixed.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Average numbers of C.P. selections in different choices and treatments before and after the stimulations. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals.

Parameters

We can infer the participants’ preferences for risk and ambiguity by the estimation model in subsection “Data Analysis.” This model is much more precise in depicting participants’ behaviors than the number of C.P. selections because it considers the different payoffs and probabilities of each choice. We repeated the estimations for 100 times and chose the one with the maximum number of correct predictions as the estimated value of ρ and γ. Figure 4 shows the estimated parameters and the corresponding preferences of the participants before and after the stimulation. The plot region is divided into four quadrants, representing four combinations of risk and ambiguity preference. Each marker represents a single participant. We note that in Figure 4 the majority of the participants are located near the middle of the plot region in the lower left quadrant, meaning that they are slightly ambiguity preferring and risk averse. However, some participants are much more ambiguity preferring and risk averse. Spearman’s rho test indicates a significant positive relationship between the values of γ and ρ (correlation coefficient: 0.490; p < 0.001). This finding reveals that the more ambiguity preferring the participant is, the more risk averse he/she is, and vice versa.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. Estimated parameters and the corresponding preferences of participants before and after the stimulations. The X-axis depicts the degree of ambiguity aversion and the Y-axis depicts the degree of risk aversion.

Figure 5 summarizes the mean values of the participants’ parameters before and after the stimulation. Again, we tested the difference across the area and stimulation type before the stimulations using one-way ANOVA with the parameter values as the dependent variable. Significant effect was observed on neither of the two parameters.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 5. Mean values of the participants’ parameters before and after the stimulations. Error bars indicate 95% confidence intervals. Asterisks indicate statistical significance of difference.

Effect of tDCS

By comparing the parameter values before and after the stimulation, we can determine whether the tDCS changed the participants’ preferences for risk and ambiguity in different treatments. We applied a repeated measures ANOVA with parameter (γ vs. ρ) and time (before vs. after stimulation) as within-subject factors, while area and stimulation type served as between-subject factors. We found a significant effect of the interaction of time and stimulation type (F2,66 = 9.655, p < 0.001). The tests of simple main effect indicated that the parameter values were higher after the right anodal/left cathodal stimulation (before: mean = 0.635; after: mean = 0.766; p = 0.001) and lower after the left anodal/right cathodal stimulation (before: mean = 0.725; after: mean = 0.623; p = 0.009). We also found a significant effect of the interaction of time and parameter (F1,66 = 4.046, p = 0.048), indicating the value of γ was significantly larger than ρ after the stimulations (γ : mean = 0.746; ρ : mean = 0.681; p = 0.018). More importantly, there was a significant effect of the interaction of parameter, time, area and stimulation type (F2,66 = 6.468, p = 0.003). Simple main effect tests showed divergent effects of different treatments on different parameters. The value of γ was significantly improved by the right anodal/left cathodal tDCS on OFC (before: mean = 0.612; after: mean = 0.869; p < 0.001), whereas it was significantly decreased by the left anodal/right cathodal tDCS on the same area (before: mean = 0.857; after: mean = 0.678; p = 0.004). Conversely, the value of ρ was significantly improved by the right anodal/left cathodal tDCS on DLPFC (before: mean = 0.551; after: mean = 0.697; p = 0.048) but significantly decreased by the reversed tDCS on the same area (before: mean = 0.753; after: mean = 0.582; p = 0.021). In other words, the right anodal/left cathodal stimulation on the OFC made the participants more ambiguity averse and the same stimulation on the DLPFC made the participants more risk preferring, while the reversed tDCS on the two areas had the corresponding reversed effects. No significant changes in parameter values were observed in the sham stimulation.

Gender Difference

Because the gender of the participants was not perfectly balanced in our experiment (42 males and 30 females), we further checked whether there was any gender difference in participants’ behaviors that may have influenced our results. One-way ANOVA showed no significant difference between males and females in the parameter of γ (F1,70 = 0.511, p = 0.477) or the parameter of ρ (F1,70 = 0.823, p = 0.367) before the stimulation. In addition, we added gender to the repeated measures ANOVA in subsection “Effect of tDCS”; again, no significant effect of gender or its interaction with other factors was observed.

Bootstrap Errors

To test the robustness of our estimated values with respect to the participant’s selections, we drew 1000 bootstrap samples and calculated the bootstrap errors of the estimated values for each participant. The average standard deviations of the bootstrap estimations of γ are 0.178 (before) and 0.174 (after), while the average standard deviations of the bootstrap estimations of ρ are 0.127 (before) and 0.120 (after). Figure 6 displays the participants’ parameter values as well as the bootstrap errors of the 1000 bootstrap estimations. Each marker represents a single participant’s parameter value, with crosses representing the standard deviation of the 1000 bootstrap estimations. The diagonal line divides the plot region into two parts: the left part means that the mean parameter value was higher after the stimulation, and the right part means that the mean parameter value was lower after the stimulation. From the figure we can see that for some participants, the standard deviations of bootstrap estimations are non-trivial, which may weaken the robustness of our results.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 6. Parameter values and bootstrap errors before and after the stimulation in different treatments. Each marker represents a single participant’s parameter value, with crosses representing the bootstrap standard deviation.

Discussion

This study used tDCS to investigate the effect of modulating the activity of the DLPFC and OFC on participants’ preferences for two types of uncertainty, namely risk and ambiguity. We applied a pre–post experiment design and a risk/ambiguity decision-making task from which we could calculate the participants’ preference parameters via an estimation model. We found evidence regarding the different neural basis of decision making under risk and ambiguity. Modulating the activity of the DLPFC using right anodal/left cathodal tDCS significantly enhanced the participants’ preferences for risk, while modulating the activity of the OFC with right anodal/left cathodal tDCS significantly decreased the participants’ preferences for ambiguity. The reversed effect was also observed in the reversed tDCS on the two areas.

The tDCS used in our study is a non-invasive brain stimulation technique that can modulate the activity of a certain area of brain cortex. Compared to brain imaging techniques, brain stimulation can better demonstrate the causal relationship between the activation of the target area and the corresponding behavior. Numerous neuroimaging studies have indicated a significant correlation between the DLPFC and risk decisions as well as the OFC and ambiguity decisions (Bechara et al., 1996; Hsu et al., 2005; Kuhnen and Knutson, 2005; Huettel et al., 2006; Lin et al., 2014). However, it is difficult to conclude whether the participants’ preferences for risk and ambiguity are attributed to the activity of the DLPFC/OFC, or whether there are other mechanisms involved in the decision-making process that may activate the corresponding areas. Using tDCS, we were able not only to enhance but also to inhibit the activity of the target areas to examine the causal effects of the DLPFC/OFC on risk and ambiguity preferences, which can help to better understand the neural mechanism of decision-making under uncertainty.

One of the crucial experiment designs of our study is the separation of risk and ambiguity. Previous experimental studies have indicated a behavioral difference in the participants confronted with risk and ambiguity situations (Ellsberg, 1961; Frisch and Baron, 1988; Camerer and Weber, 1992; Fox and Tversky, 1995; Blankenstein et al., 2016). Furthermore, neuroimaging studies have found a different neural basis of decision-making under these two types of uncertainty (Bechara et al., 1996; Hsu et al., 2005; Kuhnen and Knutson, 2005; Huettel et al., 2006; Knoch et al., 2006; Lin et al., 2014). However, the majority of brain stimulation studies use either risk tasks such as the Rogers’ Risk Task (Rogers et al., 1999) and the risk measurement table, or ambiguity tasks such as the Balloon Analogue Risk Task in which it is difficult to compare how the same stimulation will have different effects on the preferences for risk and ambiguity. Therefore, we applied the risk/ambiguity decision-making task to separate the participants’ preferences for risk and ambiguity. The task is modified from Hsu et al. (2005) and can delicately separate the two types of preference through the participants’ selections with an estimation model. We found significantly different effects of the DLPFC and OFC on participants’ risk and ambiguity attitudes: stimulation of the DLPFC is responsible for the attitude change in risk situations but not in ambiguity situations, while stimulation of the OFC is responsible for the attitude change in ambiguity situations but not in risk situations.

The close relationship between the DLPFC and risk behavior has been demonstrated by various brain imaging and brain stimulation studies (Bechara et al., 1996; Hsu et al., 2005; Kuhnen and Knutson, 2005; Huettel et al., 2006; Knoch et al., 2006; Fecteau et al., 2007a,b; Rao et al., 2008; Lin et al., 2014; Ye et al., 2015a,b, 2016; Huang et al., 2017). In our study, we found that enhancing the activity of the right DLPFC while inhibiting the activity of the left DLPFC significantly increased the participants’ preferences for risk, which is consistent with previous conclusions (Ye et al., 2015a,b). Meanwhile, we found that inhibiting the activity of the right DLPFC while enhancing the activity of the left DLPFC significantly decreased the participants’ preferences for risk. The right DLPFC has been indicated to be associated with cognitive regulation of emotions, especially the regulation of negative emotions and impulsive choice inhibition (Davidson et al., 2000; Ersche et al., 2005; Shackman et al., 2009; Schonberg et al., 2011; Yamamoto et al., 2015), and emotions are strongly involved in the risk decision process (Boggio et al., 2009; Mohr et al., 2010; Martin and Delgado, 2011; Peña-Gómez et al., 2011; Cohn et al., 2015; Engelmann et al., 2015). In this case, enhancing the right DLPFC may help to regulate or suppress the negative emotions such as the fear of failure and may inspire the participants to choose riskier behaviors.

Conversely, we found that enhancing the activity of the right OFC while inhibiting the activity of the left OFC significantly decreased the participants’ preferences for ambiguity, while the reversed montage had the reversed effect. Studies have found that the OFC is more active in situations of ambiguity than in situations of risk, and OFC-lesioned participants are not sensitive to the degrees of ambiguity and risk (Bechara et al., 1996; Hsu et al., 2005). The laterality in OFC is also reported as stronger effects are observed in the right OFC than in the left part during risk/ambiguity decision making (Hsu et al., 2005), and other studies found evidences of the lateralization of the activity within the right OFC (Milner, 1972; Tulving et al., 1994; Courtney et al., 1996; Haxby et al., 1996). Nevertheless, the laterality of OFC in risk/ambiguity decision-making remains seldom discussed. As an integration of the OFC in receiving emotional and cognitive input from the limbic system is implicated (Critchley et al., 2001; Hsu et al., 2005), our results indicate that the right OFC may play a more significant role in perceiving the negative emotions that occur during the ambiguity decision-making process, which inspires the participants to choose more ambiguity-averse behaviors. However, it is difficult to explain why the mechanism of emotion suppression of the DLPFC does not apply to ambiguity situations and why the mechanism of emotion receiving of the OFC does not apply to risk situations. Further investigations are needed to reveal the underlying neural difference between risk and ambiguity decision-making processes.

While the distinct effects of DLPFC and OFC on risk and ambiguity attitudes observed in our study is intriguing, it seems to be opposite to the meta-analysis by Krain et al. (2006) which reveals a significant association of DLPFC with ambiguity and an association of OFC with risk. One of the possible explanations for this inconsistency may be the difference in experimental paradigm. This study applies a risk/ambiguity decision-making task modified from Hsu et al. (2005) which can test the participants’ preferences for pure risk and entire ambiguity at the same time via simple either-or choices. However, the studies in the meta-analysis use several different tasks such as the Iowa Gambling Task, which is difficult to conclude whether it belongs to risk decision-making or ambiguity decision-making (Bechara and Damasio, 2005; Brand et al., 2007). In addition, some of the studies focus on unusual participants (e.g., methamphetamine dependent subjects and schizophrenia subjects) that may have neural differences compared to the participants in our study. Another concern is that the studies in the meta-analysis use either functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) or positron emission tomography, which has quite different functional mechanism from tDCS. Krain et al. (2006) also mentions the limitations of fMRI in exploring median OFC’s activation as well as the potential bias resulting from the methodological limitations and the inclusion of the same investigator’s studies. All mentioned may lead to the inconsistent conclusions between Krain et al. (2006) and our study.

In our study, the majority of the participants were slightly ambiguity preferring and risk averse. Meanwhile, the more ambiguity preferring the participant is, the more risk averse he/she is, and vice versa. The participants’ risk preferences here are consistent with previous studies showing that people are risk averse when making decisions with positive outcomes (Kahneman and Tversky, 1979, 1984; Cohen et al., 1987; Smith et al., 2002; De Martino et al., 2006). Nevertheless, certain studies suggest that people are more willing to bet on risky outcomes than on ambiguous ones for the same benefits, which is known as ambiguity aversion (Ellsberg, 1961; Camerer and Weber, 1992). In addition, risk and ambiguity aversion are found to be positively correlated among investors (Butler et al., 2013). Our finding may be partly attributed to the different task and definition applied in our study because some studies displayed a reasonable impact of ambiguity type on attitudes (Chew et al., 2008, 2011). However, some researchers did find that participants mostly had ambiguity-neutral attitudes and seldom had ambiguity-averse or -preferring attitudes (Charness et al., 2013). Behavioral experiments in China have also found a wide range of attitudes, from ambiguity preferring to strong ambiguity aversion, among investors (Potamites and Zhang, 2012). In general, additional investigations are needed to verify people’s ambiguity attitudes and their relationship with risk attitudes, which could be a limitation of our study.

Another limitation would be the focality of tDCS. The technical constrains of tDCS mean that it is difficult to control the current flow and conclude whether the observed effects were due to selective modulation of the target area or rather due to the inevitable widespread and non-selective modulation over the cortex (Sellaro et al., 2016). The focality of our stimulation on DLPFC and OFC may be further limited by using the large electrodes and the bipolar scalp electrode arrangement (Gandiga et al., 2006; Nitsche et al., 2008). More convincing investigations would be those reducing the sizes of the electrodes to obtain more spatially limited excitability modification or applying an extracephalic return electrode as the reference (Accornero et al., 2007; Nitsche et al., 2007; Ferrucci et al., 2008). Furthermore, our bootstrap sampling indicated non-trivial standard deviations for some participants, which may weaken the robustness of our results. In this sense, our study provides some inspirations about the distinct roles of DLPFC and OFC on risk and ambiguity preferences, but still needs more robust evidences from studies in the future.

To some extent, our results suggest that decision-making processes under risk and ambiguity are very complicated and may be encoded in two distinct circuits in our brains; the DLPFC mainly affects decisions under risk, while the OFC affects decisions under ambiguity. Because decision-making under uncertainty is common in life, it is meaningful to explore its neural function to obtain a better understanding of people’s uncertainty decision behaviors and various cognitive diseases. Future studies may include the neural mechanism of the distinct effects of the DLPFC and OFC on risk and ambiguity decision-making processes as well as the behavioral relationship between the two types of uncertainty attitudes.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Zhejiang University ethics committee with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Zhejiang University ethics committee.

Author Contributions

XY, MG, JS, HY, and SC designed the experiment. MG and SC performed the experiment. XY and SC analyzed the data. MG and HY drew the figures. XY, MG, JS, HY, and SC wrote the manuscript, revised the manuscript and finally approved the version to be published.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the National Social Science Fund, China (Grant number: 13AZD061, 15ZDB134), the National Nature Science Foundation of China (Grant number: 71403242), the Social Science Planning Fund Program of Zhejiang Province of China (Grant number: 16NDJC156YB) and the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities. The authors thank the reviewers for the insightful suggestions. Thanks also go to Xiaofeng Chen for help in program optimizing as well as Yiyang Lin and Ziqing Chen for help in experiment performing.

References

Accornero, N., Voti, P. L., La Riccia, M., and Gregori, B. (2007). Visual evoked potentials modulation during direct current cortical polarization. Exp. Brain Res. 178, 261–266. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0733-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bechara, A., and Damasio, A. R. (2005). The somatic marker hypothesis: a neural theory of economic decision. Games Econ. Behav. 52, 336–372. doi: 10.1016/j.geb.2004.06.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bechara, A., Tranel, D., Damasio, H., and Damasio, A. R. (1996). Failure to respond autonomically to anticipated future outcomes following damage to prefrontal cortex. Cereb. Cortex 6, 215–225. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.2.215

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Blankenstein, N. E., Crone, E. A., Den Bos, W. V., and Van Duijvenvoorde, A. C. (2016). Dealing with uncertainty: testing risk- and ambiguity-attitude across adolescence. Dev. Neuropsychol. 41, 77–92. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2016.1158265

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boggio, P., Zaghi, S., and Fregni, F. (2009). Modulation of emotions associated with images of human pain using anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Neuropsychologia 47, 212–217. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bouyssou, D., and Marchant, T. (2011). Subjective expected utility without preferences. J. Math. Psychol. 55, 457–468. doi: 10.1016/j.jmp.2011.08.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brand, M., Recknor, E. C., Grabenhorst, F., and Bechara, A. (2007). Decisions under ambiguity and decisions under risk: correlations with executive functions and comparisons of two different gambling tasks with implicit and explicit rules. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 29, 86–99. doi: 10.1080/13803390500507196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butler, J. V., Guiso, L., and Jappelli, T. (2013). The role of intuition and reasoning in driving aversion to risk and ambiguity. Theory Decis. 77, 455–484. doi: 10.1007/s11238-013-9407-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Camerer, C. F., and Weber, M. (1992). Recent developments in modeling preferences: uncertainty and ambiguity. J. Risk Uncert. 5, 325–370. doi: 10.1007/BF00122575

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charness, G., Karni, E., and Levin, D. (2013). Ambiguity attitudes and social interactions: an experimental investigation. J. Risk Uncert. 46, 1–25. doi: 10.1007/s11166-012-9157-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chew, S. H., Ebstein, R. P., and Zhong, S. (2011). Ambiguity aversion and familiarity bias: evidence from behavioral and gene association studies. J. Risk Uncert. 44, 1–18. doi: 10.1007/s11166-011-9134-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chew, S. H., Li, K. K., Chark, R., and Zhong, S. (2008). Source preference and ambiguity aversion: models and evidence from behavioral and neuroimaging experiments. Adv. Health Econ. Health Serv. Res. 20, 179–201. doi: 10.1016/S0731-2199(08)20008-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, M., Jaffray, J., and Said, T. (1987). Experimental comparison of individual behavior under risk and under uncertainty for gains and for losses. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 39, 1–22. doi: 10.1016/0749-5978(87)90043-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohn, A., Engelmann, J., Fehr, E., and Maréchal, M. A. (2015). Evidence for countercyclical risk aversion: an experiment with financial professionals. Am. Econ. Rev. 105, 860–885. doi: 10.1257/aer.20131314

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Courtney, S. M., Ungerleider, L. G., Keil, K., and Haxby, J. V. (1996). Object and spatial visual working memory activate separate neural systems in human cortex. Cereb. Cortex 6, 39–49. doi: 10.1093/cercor/6.1.39

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Critchley, H. D., Mathias, C. J., and Dolan, R. J. (2001). Neural activity in the human brain relating to uncertainty and arousal during anticipation. Neuron 29, 537–545. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00225-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Curley, S. P., and Yates, J. F. (1985). The center and range of the probability interval as factors affecting ambiguity preferences. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 36, 273–287. doi: 10.1016/0749-5978(85)90016-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davidson, R. J., Putnam, K. M., and Larson, C. L. (2000). Dysfunction in the neural circuitry of emotion regulation—a possible prelude to violence. Science 289, 591–594. doi: 10.1126/science.289.5479.591

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Martino, B., Kumaran, D., Seymour, B., and Dolan, R. J. (2006). Frames, biases, and rational decision-making in the human brain. Science 313, 684–687. doi: 10.1126/science.1128356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellsberg, D. (1961). Risk, ambiguity, and the savage axioms. Q. J. Econ. 75, 643–669. doi: 10.2307/1884324

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engelmann, J. B., Meyer, F., Fehr, E., and Ruff, C. C. (2015). Anticipatory anxiety disrupts neural valuation during risky choice. J. Neurosci. 35, 3085–3099. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2880-14.2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ersche, K., Fletcher, P., Lewis, S., Clark, L., Stocks-Gee, G., London, M., et al. (2005). Abnormal frontal activations related to decision-making in current and former amphetamine and opiate dependent individuals. Psychopharmacology 180, 612–623. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-2205-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fecteau, S., Knoch, D., Fregni, F., Sultani, N., Boggio, P. S., and Pascualleone, A. (2007a). Diminishing risk-taking behavior by modulating activity in the prefrontal cortex: a direct current stimulation study. J. Neurosci. 27, 12500–12505. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3283-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fecteau, S., Pascualleone, A., Zald, D. H., Liguori, P., Theoret, H., Boggio, P. S., et al. (2007b). Activation of prefrontal cortex by transcranial direct current stimulation reduces appetite for risk during ambiguous decision making. J. Neurosci. 27, 6212–6218. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0314-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferrucci, R., Marceglia, S., Vergari, M., Cogiamanian, F., Mrakicsposta, S., Mameli, F., et al. (2008). Cerebellar transcranial direct current stimulation impairs the practice-dependent proficiency increase in working memory. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 20, 1687–1697. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2008.20112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischbacher, U. (2007). Z-Tree. Zurich toolbox for readymade economic experiments. Exp. Econ. 10, 171–178. doi: 10.1007/s10683-006-9159-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fox, C. R., and Tversky, A. (1995). Ambiguity aversion and comparative ignorance. Q. J. Econ. 110, 585–603. doi: 10.2307/2946693

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frisch, D., and Baron, J. (1988). Ambiguity and rationality. J. Behav. Decis. Making 1, 149–157. doi: 10.1002/bdm.3960010303

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gandiga, P. C., Hummel, F. C., and Cohen, L. G. (2006). Transcranial DC stimulation (tDCS): a tool for double-blind sham-controlled clinical studies in brain stimulation. Clin. Neurophysiol. 117, 845–850. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2005.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haxby, J. V., Ungerleider, L. G., Horwitz, B., Maisog, J. M., Rapoport, S. I., and Grady, C. L. (1996). Face encoding and recognition in the human brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93, 922–927. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.2.922

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hsu, M., Bhatt, M., Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., and Camerer, C. F. (2005). Neural systems responding to degrees of uncertainty in human decision-making. Science 310, 1680–1683. doi: 10.1126/science.1115327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huang, D., Chen, S., Wang, S., Shi, J., Ye, H., Luo, J., et al. (2017). Activation of the DLPFC reveals an asymmetric effect in risky decision making: evidence from a tDCS study. Front. Psychol. 8:38. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huettel, S. A., Stowe, C. J., Gordon, E. M., Warner, B. T., and Platt, M. L. (2006). Neural signatures of economic preferences for risk and ambiguity. Neuron 49, 765–775. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.01.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahneman, D., and Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica 47, 263–291. doi: 10.2307/1914185

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahneman, D., and Tversky, A. (1984). Choices, values, and frames. Am. Psychol. 39, 341–350. doi: 10.1037//0003-066X.39.4.341

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knoch, D., Gianotti, L. R., Pascualleone, A., Treyer, V., Regard, M., Hohmann, M., et al. (2006). Disruption of right prefrontal cortex by low-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation induces risk-taking behavior. J. Neurosci. 26, 6469–6472. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0804-06.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krain, A. L., Wilson, A. M., Arbuckle, R., Castellanos, F. X., and Milham, M. P. (2006). Distinct neural mechanisms of risk and ambiguity: a meta-analysis of decision-making. NeuroImage 32, 477–484. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhberger, A., and Perner, J. (2003). The role of competition and knowledge in the Ellsberg task. J. Behav. Decis. Mak. 16, 181–191. doi: 10.1002/bdm.441

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuhnen, C. M., and Knutson, B. (2005). The neural basis of financial risk taking. Neuron 47, 763–770. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, Z. J., Li, L., Cazzell, M., and Liu, H. (2014). Atlas-guided volumetric diffuse optical tomography enhanced by generalized linear model analysis to image risk decision-making responses in young adults. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 4249–4266. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22459

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, L. N., and Delgado, M. R. (2011). The influence of emotion regulation on decision-making under risk. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 2569–2581. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2011.21618

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Merzagora, A., Foffani, G., Panyavin, I., Mordillomateos, L., Aguilar, J., Onaral, B., et al. (2010). Prefrontal hemodynamic changes produced by anodal direct current stimulation. NeuroImage 49, 2304–2310. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Milner, B. (1972). Disorders of learning and memory after temporal lobe lesions in man. Clin. Neurosurg. 19, 421–446.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Mohr, P. N., Biele, G., and Heekeren, H. R. (2010). Neural processing of risk. J. Neurosci. 30, 6613–6619. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0003-10.2010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitsche, M. A., Cohen, L. G., Wassermann, E. M., Priori, A., Lang, N., Antal, A., et al. (2008). Transcranial direct current stimulation: state of the art 2008. Brain Stimul. 1, 206–223. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2008.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitsche, M. A., and Paulus, W. (2000). Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J. Physiol. 527, 633–639. doi: 10.1212/WNL.57.10.1899

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nitsche, M. A., Roth, A., Kuo, M. F., Fischer, A. K., Liebetanz, D., Lang, N., et al. (2007). Timing-dependent modulation of associative plasticity by general network excitability in the human motor cortex. J. Neurosci. 27, 3807–3812.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Ouellet, J., Mcgirr, A., Van den Eynde, F., Jollant, F., Lepage, M., and Berlim, M. T. (2015). Enhancing decision-making and cognitive impulse control with transcranial direct current stimulation (tdcs) applied over the orbitofrontal cortex (ofc): a randomized and sham-controlled exploratory study. J. Psychiatr. Res. 69, 27–34. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.07.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peña-Gómez, C., Vidal-Piñeiro, D., Clemente, I. C., Pascual-Leone,Á, and Bartrés-Faz, D. (2011). Down-regulation of negative emotional processing by transcranial direct current stimulation: effects of personality characteristics. PLoS ONE 6:e22812. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0022812

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Potamites, E., and Zhang, B. (2012). Heterogeneous ambiguity attitudes: a field experiment among small-scale stock investors in China. Rev. Econ. Design 16, 193–213. doi: 10.1007/s10058-012-0125-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rao, H., Korczykowski, M., Pluta, J., Hoang, A., and Detre, J. A. (2008). Neural correlates of voluntary and involuntary risk taking in the human brain: an fMRI study of the balloon analog risk task (BART). NeuroImage 42, 902–910. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.05.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, R. D., Owen, A. M., Middleton, H. C., Williams, E. J., Pickard, J. D., Sahakian, B. J., et al. (1999). Choosing between small, likely rewards and large, unlikely rewards activates inferior and orbital prefrontal cortex. J. Neurosci. 19, 9029–9038.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Savage, L. (1954/1972). The Foundations of Statistics. New York: John Wiley and Sons.

Google Scholar

Schonberg, T., Fox, C. R., and Poldrack, R. A. (2011). Mind the gap: bridging economic and naturalistic risk-taking with cognitive neuroscience. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 11–19. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sellaro, R., Nitsche, M. A., and Colzato, L. S. (2016). The stimulated social brain: effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on social cognition. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1369, 218–239. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shackman, A. J., McMenamin, B. W., Maxwell, J. S., Greischar, L. L., and Davidson, R. J. (2009). Right dorsolateral prefrontal cortical activity and behavioral inhibition. Psychol. Sci. 20, 1500–1506. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02476.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, K., Dickhaut, J., Mccabe, K., and Pardo, J. V. (2002). Neuronal substrates for choice under ambiguity, risk, gains, and losses. Manage. Sci. 48, 711–718. doi: 10.1287/mnsc.48.6.711.194

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tulving, E., Markowitsch, H. J., Kapur, S., Habib, R., and Houle, S. (1994). Novelty encoding networks in the human brain: positron emission tomography data. Neuroreport 5, 2525–2528. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199412000-00030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, G., Li, J., Yin, X., Li, S., and Wei, M. (2016). Modulating activity in the orbitofrontal cortex changes trustees’ cooperation: a transcranial direct current stimulation study. Behav. Brain Res. 303, 71–75. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2016.01.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Willis, M. L., Murphy, J. M., Ridley, N. J., and Vercammen, A. (2015). Anodal tDCS targeting the right orbitofrontal cortex enhances facial expression recognition. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10, 1677–1683. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsv057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yamamoto, D., Woo, C., Wager, T., Regner, M., and Tanabe, J. (2015). Influence of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and ventral striatum on risk avoidance in addiction: a mediation analysis. Drug Alcohol Depend. 149, 10–17. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.12.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ye, H., Chen, S., Huang, D., Wang, S., Jia, Y., and Luo, J. (2015a). Transcranial direct current stimulation over prefrontal cortex diminishes degree of risk aversion. Neurosci. Lett. 598, 18–22. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2015.04.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ye, H., Chen, S., Huang, D., Wang, S., and Luo, J. (2015b). Modulating activity in the prefrontal cortex changes decision-making for risky gains and losses: a transcranial direct current stimulation study. Behav. Brain Res. 286, 17–21. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.02.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ye, H., Huang, D., Wang, S., Zheng, H., Luo, J., and Chen, S. (2016). Activation of the prefrontal cortex by unilateral transcranial direct current stimulation leads to an asymmetrical effect on risk preference in frames of gain and loss. Brain Res. 1648(Pt A), 325–332. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: risk decision-making, ambiguity decision-making, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, orbital frontal cortex, transcranial direct current stimulation

Citation: Yang X, Gao M, Shi J, Ye H and Chen S (2017) Modulating the Activity of the DLPFC and OFC Has Distinct Effects on Risk and Ambiguity Decision-Making: A tDCS Study. Front. Psychol. 8:1417. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01417

Received: 07 March 2017; Accepted: 04 August 2017;
Published: 22 August 2017.

Edited by:

Paul E. M. Phillips, University of Washington, United States

Reviewed by:

John Pearson, Duke University, United States
Karli K. Watson, University of Colorado Boulder, United States

Copyright © 2017 Yang, Gao, Shi, Ye and Chen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Shu Chen, zjuchenshu@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.