Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 25 August 2017
Sec. Developmental Psychology

Parent Rated Symptoms of Inattention in Childhood Predict High School Academic Achievement Across Two Culturally and Diagnostically Diverse Samples

  • 1Department of Biological and Medical Psychology, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway
  • 2K. G. Jebsen Center for Neuropsychiatric Disorders, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway
  • 3Department of Psychology, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA, United States
  • 4Regional Centre for Child and Youth Mental Health and Child Welfare, Uni Research Health, Bergen, Norway
  • 5Department of Psychiatry, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, United States

Objective: To investigate parent reports of childhood symptoms of inattention as a predictor of adolescent academic achievement, taking into account the impact of the child’s intellectual functioning, in two diagnostically and culturally diverse samples.

Method: Samples: (a) an all-female sample in the U.S. predominated by youth with ADHD (Berkeley Girls with ADHD Longitudinal Study [BGALS], N = 202), and (b) a mixed-sex sample recruited from a Norwegian population-based sample (the Bergen Child Study [BCS], N = 93). Inattention and intellectual function were assessed via the same measures in the two samples; academic achievement scores during and beyond high school and demographic covariates were country-specific.

Results: Childhood inattention predicted subsequent academic achievement in both samples, with a somewhat stronger effect in the BGALS sample, which included a large subgroup of children with ADHD. Intellectual function was another strong predictor, but the effect of early inattention remained statistically significant in both samples when intellectual function was covaried.

Conclusion: The effect of early indicators of inattention on future academic success was robust across the two samples. These results support the use of remediation procedures broadly applied. Future longitudinal multicenter studies with pre-planned common inclusion criteria should be performed to increase our understanding of the importance of inattention in primary school children for concurrent and prospective functioning.

Introduction

Academic achievement is an important predictor of later vocational career success and adult financial stability (Richardson et al., 2012; Fried et al., 2016). Identification of predictors of academic success is therefore of great importance. The link between externalizing behavior and academic underachievement is well-documented (Hinshaw, 1992a), particularly in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (Hinshaw, 1992b; Loe and Feldman, 2007; Spencer et al., 2007; Daley and Birchwood, 2010; Owens and Jackson, 2017). Other studies have emphasized the importance of inattention, one of the core symptom domains of ADHD. This association is illustrated by longitudinal follow-up studies of girls participating in the Berkeley Girls with ADHD Longitudinal Study (BGALS; see Lee and Hinshaw, 2006; Owens et al., 2017) and substantiated by Garner et al. (2013). In the latter investigation, based on teacher and parent ratings of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity symptoms in a large sample of 5663 boys and girls with ADHD (age range: 3–17), teacher ratings of inattention emerged as the strongest predictor of later academic achievement.

Symptoms of inattention, however, are not restricted to a specific diagnostic group. Most patients with neuropsychiatric disorders (e.g., Carmichael et al., 2015), as well as a proportion of the general population (e.g., Polderman et al., 2007), display some degree of problems related to inattention. In a systematic review of prospective studies, Polderman et al. (2010) confirmed that inattention is a core risk factor for poor academic achievement in both clinical and population-based samples. Pingault et al. (2011) performed an in-depth study of developmental trajectories in a 16-year longitudinal study of a French-Canadian community sample. They found that the risk of graduation failure dramatically increased in boys and girls who displayed rising rates of inattention during the period from childhood through adolescence. A population-based Swedish study by Holmberg and Bolte (2014) showed that a high risk for academic failure in upper secondary school (16 years of age) could be directly predicted from teacher-reported problems related to inattention in primary school (7 years of age). A direct effect of inattention to lowered academic achievement was also convincingly shown in a longitudinal study including a community sample of elementary school children aged 5–7 years (Gray et al., 2015). Furthermore, a recent investigation showed that the association between inattentive behavior in kindergarten and academic function in eighth grade was strong among children with both severe and less severe ADHD (Owens and Jackson, 2017). Taken together, these findings suggest that inattention in early childhood predicts later academic problems across diverse samples. Results from different studies are, however, difficult to compare because of cultural as well as methodological differences. Through the present investigation we aim to add to this literature by including ratings of inattention and later academic achievement in two culturally and diagnostically diverse samples—yet with key methodological similarities, which afford direct comparison of findings.

We note that most previous studies in this area feature teacher ratings of inattention, often to the exclusion of parent ratings of this variable (e.g., Pingault et al., 2011; Rogers et al., 2011). In addition, reflecting longstanding trends across all of child and adolescent psychopathology (Achenbach et al., 1987), agreement between teacher and parent ratings of problem behavior, including inattention, is low in both ADHD (Mitsis et al., 2000; Murray et al., 2007) and typically developing samples (Mares et al., 2007). A possible explanation is that teachers and parents may rate different aspects of inattention that occur in different environments (i.e., in school vs. at home). Because less is known about how parent’s ratings of inattention predict later academic achievement, we feature this information source in the present report.

Items used to assess inattention are closely related to cognitive/executive functions like flexibility, inhibition, and working memory, and are thus related to broader issues of executive function, as articulated in the classic review of Barkley (1997). This pattern suggests that a direct effect of inattention on academic achievement may be mediated or explained by key aspects of cognitive function. For example, Gray et al. (2015) found that the direct effect of early inattention on later academic performance was partially mediated by a test of working memory. Given that aspects of dysfunctional cognition are described as a core component of ADHD (e.g., Sonuga-Barke, 2005; Barkley et al., 2008), an additive effect of inattention and cognitive dysfunction may be expected within this diagnostic group. Indeed, in adults with ADHD, those with co-existent cognitive dysfunction obtain lower academic success than those without (Biederman et al., 2006; Halleland et al., 2015). Together, these studies emphasize the importance of taking cognitive function into account when predicting academic achievement from early inattention symptoms.

Selection of cognitive measures is, however, challenging. Cognitive function may be defined from a single test or clusters of tests designed to assess the parallel cognitive domains or subfunctions (see, e.g., Miyake et al., 2000; Salthouse et al., 2003). It may also be defined as an overall “g” (or general) factor. Arguments for the latter are based on the moderate to strong correlation across tests of different cognitive processes. Intellectual function represents such an overall g-factor, revealing strong predictive associations to academic performance (Deary, 2012). We therefore include intellectual function as a proxy for overall cognitive function in this report.

Current Study

We investigate the relation between parent-rated inattention in primary school children and their future academic success in high school by including the following two culturally and diagnostically diverse samples: (a) the Berkeley Girls with ADHD Longitudinal Study (BGALS), comprising girls with an ADHD diagnosis and a matched comparison group who attend or have attended high school (see Hinshaw, 2002; Hinshaw et al., 2012); and (b) a sample recruited from the population-based Bergen Child Study (BCS, Heiervang et al., 2007), including high-school youth. Based on analyses of these two datasets, we investigate the strength of the inattention-academic achievement link in the two samples, as well as the influence of intellectual function on this link.

In both samples, parents rated their children’s inattentive behavior according to the nine inattention items from the Swanson, Nolan, and Pelham Questionnaire (SNAP-IV, Swanson, 1992). Intellectual function was assessed via the third edition of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-III; Wechsler, 1991). Information about academic achievement was assessed during adolescence or early adulthood according to national standards. Both the BGALS and BCS leverage a prospective longitudinal design, with information from two study waves being examined for the present paper: a wave in childhood and an approximately 10-year follow-up wave, at the end of adolescence. From previous studies, reviewed above, we expect to find a strong link between parent reports of inattention and future academic achievement in both samples, with the strongest effect of inattention in the BGALS, because this sample has a majority of youth with a formal ADHD diagnosis. A close relation between intellectual function and parent reports of inattention is also expected to be stronger in the BGALS than in the BCS sample. We predict that adjusting for childhood IQ will reduce the predictive effect of inattention on later academic performance to a non-significant level in the BGALS – because of its large ADHD subsample – but that inattention will remain as a significant predictor in the BCS sample, which is drawn from the general population.

Materials and Methods

Sample 1: Berkeley Girls with ADHD Longitudinal Study

Girls aged 6–12 (born 1984–1992) were recruited from schools, mental health centers, pediatric practices, and direct advertisements to participate in summer research programs in the San Francisco Bay Area, California, United States in 1997, 1998, and 1999. After initial screening, participants with ADHD were included at Wave 1 (ages 6–12 years, mean = 9.6, SD = 1.7) if they met full ADHD diagnostic criteria for the combined or inattentive types on the parent-administered Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children (DISC-IV; Shaffer et al., 2000). Disorders that commonly co-occur with ADHD (i.e., oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder, anxiety disorders, depression, and learning disorders) were allowed to enhance the generalizability of the sample. Comparison participants were matched to the ADHD sample on age and ethnicity but could not meet diagnostic criteria for ADHD on parent ratings or structured clinical interview. Exclusionary criteria for both groups were intellectual disability, pervasive developmental disorders, psychosis or overt neurological disorder, non-English spoken in the home, and a medical problem prohibiting summer camp participation.

Hundred and forty girls with and 88 age- and ethnicity-matched comparison girls without ADHD were selected after extensive screening (including the SNAP-IV; Swanson, 1992) and full diagnostic assessments (Hinshaw, 2002), including a test of intellectual function. Participants were invited to take part in prospective follow-up assessments 5 years later (age 11–18 years, mean = 14.2, SD = 1.6; see Hinshaw et al., 2006) and then 10 years later (age 17–24 years, mean = 19.6, SD = 1.7; see Hinshaw et al., 2012). Information about late-adolescent academic achievement at the 10-year follow-up is included in the present study. Aided by the use of social media in some cases, we located 216 of the 228 participants from the initial study-wave (95%). Based on 23 statistical comparisons to the retained sample, the12 participants lost to follow-up had lower family incomes and full-scale IQ scores and higher teacher-rated ADHD, externalizing, and internalizing symptoms, suggesting that the group lost to follow-up was more cognitively and behaviorally impaired.

The present sample includes girls with parent-reported information during Wave 1 on the inattention measure as well as performance on a well-standardized test of intellectual function. The measures of academic performance were obtained during Wave 3. The final sample comprised 202 children with complete data, including 128 with ADHD. All assessment waves received full approval from the institution’s Committee for the Protection of Human Subjects.

Sample 2: Bergen Child Study

The BCS is a longitudinal, population-based study on mental health and development, including three age cohorts of children born between 1993 and 1995. When launched in 2002, it included all children attending any school in the city of Bergen, the second largest city of Norway. During the initial screening phase, a four-page BCS questionnaire was given to both the parents and teachers of the target population, including, among other scales, a slightly modified SNAP-IV (Swanson, 1992). In a second phase of the first study wave, a subset of parents was interviewed using the Development and Well-Being Assessment (DAWBA) (Goodman et al., 2000). All children who obtained any diagnosis according to DAWBA and an equal subgroup of children without a diagnosis were invited to participate together with their parents in a thorough clinical examination, designed to resemble a clinical psychiatric examination. A total of 329 children participated in this clinical study, which included a diagnostic interview: the Schedule of Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Present and Lifetime version (K-SADS-PL) (Kaufman et al., 1997), plus the WISC-III (Wechsler, 1991). A fourth wave of the study invited a more comprehensive sample of all adolescents born between 1993 and 1995 living in the county of Hordaland, which includes the city of Bergen. The BCS sample was thus nested within this young@hordaland sample.

The sample for the present study includes participants with parent reports on inattentive behavior from the initial study wave (mean age = 8.14, SD = 0.81), results on a test of intellectual function performed about 1 year after the parent reports (mean age = 9.59, SD = 0.95), and an academic achievement score from the School registry when the participants attended high school (age 16–19 years, n = 104). To make the sample similar to the BGALS, 11 participants with an IQ score below 70 were excluded, together with one child defined as an outlier (>3 SD) according to casewise statistics. In total, 92 participants are included in the final BCS sample for this report. According to the Kiddie-SADS interview in the clinical phase of the first study wave, diagnosis was confirmed in 27 of the children (ADHD n = 9; anxiety n = 10; depression/dysthymia n = 2; OCD n = 1; Tourette syndrome/chronic tics n = 1; enuresis/encopresis n = 4).

The Regional Centre for Child and Youth Mental Health and Child Welfare, Uni Research, collaborated with Hordaland County Council to conduct the study. The BCS was approved by the Regional Committee for Medical and Health Research Ethics (REC), Western Norway. Parents gave written consent for participation when the children were below 16 years, and the adolescents consented themselves to participate when they were above 16 in accordance with Norwegian regulations.

Measures

Parent Ratings of Inattention

Parents completed the nine relevant items from SNAP-IV (Swanson, 1992) in both the BGALS and BCS. The items cover the symptoms defined in the inattentive-disorganized dimension of ADHD, as described in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV, American Psychiatric Association, 2000). SNAP-IV uses four levels to evaluate each item, and these levels were included in the BGALS questionnaires. Parents rated each item with a value of 0 (“not at all”), 1 (“a little”), 2 (“pretty much”), or 3 (“very much”). We calculated a sum score (range = 0 to 27) for current analyses. The BCS used a three-level item Likert-type scale (0 = “not true,” 1 = “somewhat true,” or 2 = “certainly true”), consistent with the response metric of the remaining scales included in Wave 1 of the BCS. Thus, sum scores ranged from 0 to 18.

Intellectual Functioning

Full-scale IQ (FSIQ) was calculated from the WISC-III, scored according to American norms in the BGALS study (Wechsler, 1991) and according to Swedish norms for the BCS (Sonnander et al., 1998). The WISC-III is an extensively used measure of general cognitive abilities for children and adolescents, possessing good psychometric properties (Kaufman, 1994). A trained graduate student or test technician administered the full WISC-III to all participants. The WISC-III was administrated at the same time as the parent reports of inattention in the BGALS, when the children were 6–12 years old, and about 1 year after the parent reports in BCS, when the children were 8–10 years old.

Academic Achievement

The Wechsler Individual Achievement Test (WIAT; Wechsler, 1992) was included in the third wave of the BGALS (10 years after baseline assessment, Wave 1). The WIAT is a psychometrically sound assessment of academic achievement, with both internal consistency and test–retest reliability estimates above 0.85 for most composite scores (Wechsler, 1992). The Basic Reading and Math Reasoning scores, summed together, are included as a measure of academic functioning at follow-up.

For the BCS, academic achievement scores were provided by official registers from the Hordaland County for those who consented to the school registry linkage. In Norway, grades are assigned on a scale ranging from 1 to 6, with 6 being the highest grade (outstanding competence). A score of 2 (low level of competence) is the lowest passing grade, and 1 signifies a fail. The scores represent the average of all grades except for gym class. The achievement scores from both BGALS and BCS are calculated as continuous measures.

Covariates

In the BGALS sample, several important background variables were included as covariates: (a) mother’s highest-level education, (b) total household income, (c) ethnicity, and (d) age at baseline, all of which were ascertained via a demographic questionnaire at baseline. In the BCS, covariates were restricted to age at WISC-III testing and biological sex. Among families who answered the question about mother’s education (73.1%), all but two reported at least 12 years of education. Furthermore, all but one of the participants had at least one parent born in Norway, confirming the homogeneous ethnicity of the Norwegian population. Maternal education and ethnicity were therefore not included as covariates when analyzing the BCS data.

Data Analytic Plan

Data from both studies were analyzed (SPSS version 23/24) according to the same analytic plan, including covariates that were specific to characteristics of the two countries. Group differences (between the ADHD and comparison subgroups within the BGALS and between males and females in the BCS) were initially calculated. Hierarchical linear regression analyses with academic achievement as the late-adolescent/early adult outcome measure were computed to investigate the effect of parent-reported inattention during childhood. In all analyses, relevant demographic variables were included in the first step. We then added inattention in the next step to investigate its direct effect on academic achievement. Then, to probe whether an effect of inattention was retained when intellectual function was covaried, intellectual function was included in a second step after demographics, followed by parent reports of inattention in a final step. Standardized change scores were used to reflect the unique contribution from the variables included in the regression models.

Results

Berkeley Girls with ADHD Longitudinal Study

Results are presented in Table 1. At inclusion, the ADHD group obtained significantly higher parent rated inattention scores than the comparison group (p < 0.001, d = 4.13), as well as significantly lower scores for FSIQ (p < 0.001, d = 0.94). The WIAT composite of reading and math achievement obtained during Wave 3 was far lower in the ADHD than in the comparison group (p < 0.001, d = 1.12). There were no significant group differences for the variables of age, household income, and mother’s education, but the % Caucasian was significantly higher in the ADHD group (p < 0.05).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Results in the BGALS and BCS sample.

Bivariate correlation analyses including the full sample showed statistically significant negative correlations between (a) parent reports of childhood inattention and concurrent FSIQ (r = -0.353, p < 0.001), (b) parent reports of childhood inattention and academic achievement at Wave 3 (r = -0.374, p < 0.001), and (c) childhood FSIQ and Wave 3 achievement (r = 0.687, p < 0.001). When considering the two subsamples separately, only the correlations between FSIQ and academic achievement were statistically significant, in both the ADHD (r = 0.623, p < 0.001) and comparison subsamples (r = 0.593, p < 0.001). The entire sample was therefore included in the regression analysis.

The regression analyses (see Table 2) revealed that parent-reported inattention at Wave 1 significantly predicted academic achievement scores in high school (p < 0.001), explaining 10.4% of the variance beyond the covariates (mother’s education, household income, ethnicity, and child age at baseline). Academic achievement was also significantly predicted by FSIQ (p < 0.001), explaining 36.9% of the variance beyond the covariates. When inattention was included in the final stage after entry of FSIQ, the contribution from inattention remained statistically significant (p < 0.05), bringing the overall explained variance of the model to 52.9%.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Predicting academic achievement.

Bergen Child Study

The BCS sample included a higher number of boys (n = 66) than girls (n = 26). Boys and girls obtained similar results on all measures except for parent-reported inattention, which was significantly higher in boys than in girls (p < 0.001, d = 0.76) (see Table 1). There were 14 boys and 5 girls who reported at least one psychiatric disorder at the late adolescent data collection point. ADHD was reported in only boys (n = 11), all of whom had a comorbid disorder (see Figure 1). None of the participants had psychosis, intellectual disability, or known neurological disorders.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Number of self-reported psychiatric problems in high school (BCS). Dep, depression; Eat, eating disorder; Sleep, sleep disorder; TS, Tourette syndrome; ASD, Autism/Asperger syndrome.

The correlations in the full sample were statistically significant for the relation between the childhood inattention and FSIQ scores (r = -0.273, p = 0.008), childhood inattention and the high-school academic achievement score (r = -0.402, p < 0.001), and childhood FSIQ and the high-school achievement score (r = 0.479, p = < 0.001). In a sex-specific correlation analysis, all correlations retained their statistical significance for boys (inattention/FSIQ, r = -0.293, p = 0.017; inattention/academic achievement r = -0.370, p = 0.002; FSIQ/academic achievement r = 0.532, p < 0.001), but for girls, only the correlation between inattention and academic achievement, r = -0.517, p = 0.007 remained significant. Sex was therefore kept as a covariate in the following regression analyses.

The linear regression analyses (see Table 2) revealed that parent reports of inattention significantly predicted academic achievement in high school (p = 0.001), explaining 13.8% of the variance beyond the two covariates (age and sex). Introducing intellectual function as a predictor explained 23.8% of academic achievement (p < 0.001), with 18.2% incremental variance explained by FSIQ. Inclusion of parent reports of inattention in the final step, following inclusion of the covariates and intellectual function, increased the explained variance to 30.9%, leaving the contribution from inattention statistically significant (p < 0.05).

Because the correlational pattern was different for boys and girls, the regression analysis was re-conducted separately for boys and girls with age as the only covariate. Inattention was still statistically significant for boys (p = 0.032) and girls (p = 0.008), while the effect of intellectual function was statistically significant only for boys (p < 0.001).

Discussion

In the present study, including two culturally and diagnostically diverse samples, we found a significant and replicated link between parent reports of inattention in their primary school children and future academic achievement, approximately 10 years later. The independent contribution from parent-reported inattention was remarkably similar across the two study samples when sample-specific demographic variables were covaried. The impact of intellectual function on academic achievement was also strong in both samples, particularly so in BGALS and among boys in the BCS sample. Still, the effect of parent reports of childhood inattention was significant in both samples even when childhood intellectual function was taken into account.

As noted in the introduction, several studies have emphasized the impact of early inattentive behavior on future academic function, in both clinical samples of youth with ADHD (Lee and Hinshaw, 2006; Pingault et al., 2011; Garner et al., 2013) and community samples (Holmberg and Bolte, 2014; Gray et al., 2015). The present study extended these findings by including samples representing dimensions of ADHD symptoms, allowing the study of symptom severity above and beyond diagnostic categories per se. Our results showed that negative consequences associated with inattention were not restricted to children within the diagnostic category of ADHD. Moreover, there were similar magnitudes of correlation between both intellectual functioning and inattention with respect to later achievement in the ADHD and comparison samples for BGALS, confirming that also cognitive function has a strong influence on future academic achievement in children with ADHD as well as in youth in general (Miller and Hinshaw, 2010).

The extensive covariation of socio-economic factors in the BGALS was a necessary step, in order to demonstrate specificity of predictions to later achievement from childhood inattention and IQ. However, this level of statistical adjustment was not needed in the BCS because of the socio-economic homogeneity of the Norwegian society. The importance of taking cultural differences in socio-economic status into account is illustrated in studies of intellectual function. For example, the effect of socio-economic status on intellectual function is much weaker in samples from Nordic countries than in U.S. samples (Andersson et al., 1996; Osler et al., 2013; Ellertsen et al., 2016).

Even when adjusting for childhood intellectual functioning, the incremental prediction of later achievement from childhood inattention remained as an important predictor of later academic performance. In an educational contex, this replicated finding points to the importance of remediation and training procedures aimed at helping primary school children with inattention problems. Behavioral parental training, which has been proved successful in helping preschool children (Sonuga-Barke et al., 2013), may be of importance to parents to understand and help children with problems related to inattention. Similar training programs could be offered to primary school teachers—who also require classroom aides and other supports—focusing on the risk of negative social labeling that may accompany school-related problems. Cognitive training programs have recently been increasingly popular for school children with ADHD (see, e.g., Rapport et al., 2013; Tamm et al., 2017). These types of interventions address problems that are highly relevant for inattentive behavior, including what is commonly referred to as executive dysfunction (see Colomer et al., 2017). Despite the negative findings of the review of Cortese et al. (2015) for specific cognitive training regarding youth, programs with a more general targeting of congitive and emotional function may still be found to provide some lasting benefit.

Strengths and Limitations

The inclusion of two diagnostically and culturally diverse samples, using similar methodological procedures, is a main strength of the present study. Such inclusion, however, also led to several limitations, affecting the strength of our conclusions. The selection of informant (i.e., parent reports) and the measures of cognitive function (i.e., performance on a test of intellectual function) and inattention were motivated largely by our objective of including parallel measures in both studies. Still, we believe that these selections have given us results of clinical importance. Parent reports are commonly the most available information about a child’s behavior when assessed in a clinical situation; the total score of the included questionnaire on inattention is well-validated; and the full-scale IQ score is commonly available as a general measure of cognitive function. Still, questions may be raised about the specificity of the parent reports, in that parents may find it difficult to contrast inattention with characteristics of other aspects of problematic behavior. Other limitations are related to critical differences in study designs, leading to somewhat different age groups and not quite overlapping time-lines in our longitudinal designs.

Conclusion

Inattention is one of the hallmarks of ADHD. The present study shows that features of this behavior, in particular from a parent’s perspective, are of crucial importance in predicting high-school achievement about a decade later—an effect that is not restricted to children within the diagnostic category of ADHD. Furthermore, the time span between parent reports of inattention and assessment of academic achievement was longer than in most previous studies (∼10 years), suggesting that the effects of inattention in primary school may yield consequences into adult life. Inattention tends to lead to a cascade of other problems (Gillberg, 2010; Sonuga-Barke and Halperin, 2010), including peer-related problems (see Bellanti and Bierman, 2000; Andrade and Tannock, 2014) and mood disorders (Rajendran et al., 2013; Lundervold et al., 2016). As a result, remediation of problems related to inattention may provide benefit not only for academic and later vocational problems but also for social interactions and general mental health (see Richardson et al., 2012; Fried et al., 2016).

Author Contributions

AL: data collection, design, statistical analysis and main responsibility for the manuscript; JM: data collection, design, statistical analysis and comments on the manuscript; MH: data collection, statistical analysis, comments on the manuscript; SH: data collection, design and main comments on the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The BGALS was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grant R01 MH45064, awarded to SH, as well as National Science Foundation Graduate Research Fellowship 2013172086, awarded to JM. We would also like to thank the young women who have participated in this our ongoing investigation, along with their families, and to our dedicated graduate students, staff, and research assistants.

The BCS was conducted by the Regional Centre for Child and Adolescent Mental Health and Welfare in collaboration with Hordaland County Council. The project was funded by the Norwegian Directorate for Health and Social Affairs, University of Bergen, the Norwegian Directorate for Health and Social Affairs, and the Western Norway Regional Health Authority. We are grateful to the children, parents, and teachers participating in the Bergen Child Study (BCS), as well as present and previous members of the BCS project group for making the study possible. A special thanks to Professor Gillberg who initiated the study.

The collaboration between the teams of SH and AL was made possible due to a Peder Sather Grant in 2014–2015.

References

Achenbach, T. M., McConaughy, S. H., and Howell, C. T. (1987). Child/adolescent behavioral and emotional problems: implications of cross-informant correlations for situational specificity. Psychol. Bull. 101, 213–232. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th Edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Andersson, H. W., Sommerfelt, K., Sonnander, K., and Ahlsten, G. (1996). Maternal child-rearing attitudes IQ and socioeconomic status as related to cognitive abilities of five-year-old children. Psychol. Rep. 79, 3–14. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1996.79.1.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andrade, B. F., and Tannock, R. (2014). Sustained impact of inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity on peer problems: mediating roles of prosocial skills and conduct problems in a community sample of children. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 45, 318–328. doi: 10.1007/s10578-013-0402-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol. Bull. 121, 65–94.

Google Scholar

Barkley, R. A., Murphy, K., and Fischer, M. (2008). ADHD in Adults: What the Science Says. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Google Scholar

Bellanti, C. J., and Bierman, K. L. (2000). Disentangling the impact of low cognitive ability and inattention on social behavior and peer relationships. J. Clin. Child Psychol. 29, 66–75. doi: 10.1207/S15374424jccp2901-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biederman, J., Petty, C., Fried, R., Fontanella, J., Doyle, A. E., Seidman, L. J., et al. (2006). Impact of psychometrically defined deficits of executive functioning in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am. J. Psychiatry 163, 1730–1738. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.10.1730

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carmichael, J. A., Kubas, H. A., Carlson, H. L., Fitzer, K. R., Wilcox, G., Lemay, J.-F., et al. (2015). Reconsidering “inattention” in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: implications for neuropsychological assessment and intervention. Appl. Neuropsychol. Child 4, 97–105. doi: 10.1080/21622965.2015.1005481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colomer, C., Berenguer, C., Roselló, B., Baixauli, I., and Miranda, A. (2017). The impact of inattention, hyperactivity/impulsivity Symptoms, and executive functions on learning behaviors of children with ADHD. Front. Psychol. 2:540. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00540

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cortese, S., Ferrin, M., Brandeis, D., Buitelaar, J., Daley, D., and Dittmann, R. W. (2015). Cognitive training for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: meta-analysis of clinical and neuropsychological outcomes from randomized controlled trials. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 54, 164–174. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2014.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daley, D., and Birchwood, J. (2010). ADHD and academic performance: why does ADHD impact on academic performance and what can be done to support ADHD children in the classroom? Child Care Health Dev. 36, 455–464. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2009.01046.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deary, I. J. (2012). Intelligence. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 63, 453–482. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ellertsen, T., Thorsen, A. L., Holm, S. E., Bøe, T., Sørensen, L., and Lundervold, A. J. (2016). A weak association between socioeconomic status and cognition in a sample of Norwegian children. Scand. J. Psychol. 57, 399–405. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12324

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fried, R., Petty, C., Faraone, S. V., Hyder, L. L., Day, H., and Biederman, J. (2016). Is ADHD a risk factor for high school dropout? A controlled study. J. Atten. Disord. 20, 383–389. doi: 10.1177/1087054712473180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garner, A. A., O’Connor, B. C., Narad, M. E., Tamm, L., Simon, J., and Epstein, J. N. (2013). The relationship between ADHD symptom dimensions, clinical correlates, and functional impairments. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 34, 469–477. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e3182a39890

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gillberg, C. (2010). The essence in child psychiatry: early symptomatic syndromes eliciting neurodevelopmental clinical examinations. Res. Dev. Disabil. 31, 1543–1551. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2010.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodman, R., Ford, T., Richards, H., Gatward, R., and Meltzer, H. (2000). The development and well-being assessment: description and initial validation of an integrated assessment of child and adolescent psychopathology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 41, 645–655. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2000.tb02345.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, S. A., Rogers, M., Martinussen, R., and Tannock, R. (2015). Longitudinal relations among inattention, working memory, and academic achievement: testing mediation and the moderating role of gender. PeerJ 3:e939. doi: 10.7717/peerj.939

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Halleland, H., Sørensen, L., Posserud, M., Haavik, J., and Lundervold, A. J. (2015). Occupational status is compromised in adults with ADHD and psychometrically defined executive deficits. J. Atten. Disord. doi: 10.1177/1087054714564622 [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heiervang, E., Stormark, K. M., Lundervold, A. J., Heimann, M., Goodman, R., Posserud, M. B., et al. (2007). Psychiatric disorders in norwegian 8- to 10-year-olds: an epidemiological survey of prevalence, risk factors, and service use. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 46, 438–447. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e31803062bf

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hinshaw, S. P. (1992a). Externalizing behavior problems and academic underachievement in childhood and adolescence: causal relationships and underlying mechanisms. Psychol. Bull. 111, 127–155. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.111.1.127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hinshaw, S. P. (1992b). Academic underachievement, attention deficits, and aggression: comorbidity and implications for intervention. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 60, 893–903. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.60.6.893

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hinshaw, S. P. (2002). Preadolescent girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: I. Background characteristics, comorbidity, cognitive and social functioning, and parenting practices. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 70, 1086–1098. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.70.5.1086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hinshaw, S. P., Owens, E. B., Sami, N., and Fargeon, S. (2006). Prospective follow-up of girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder into adolescence: evidence for continuing cross-domain impairment. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 74, 489–499. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.74.3.489

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hinshaw, S. P., Owens, E. B., Zalecki, C., Huggins, S. P., Montenegro-Nevado, A. J., Schrodek, E., et al. (2012). Prospective follow-up of girls with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity disorder into early adulthood: continuing impairment includes elevated risk for suicide attempts and self-injury. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 80, 1041–1051. doi: 10.1037/a0029451

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holmberg, K., and Bolte, S. (2014). Do symptoms of ADHD at ages 7 and 10 predict academic outcome at age 16 in the general population? J. Atten. Disord. 18, 635–645. doi: 10.1177/1087054712452136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaufman, A. S. (1994). Intelligent testing with the WISC-III. Oxford: John Wiley.

Google Scholar

Kaufman, J., Birmaher, B., Brent, D., Rao, U., Flynn, C., Moreci, P., et al. (1997). Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 36, 980–988. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199707000-00021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, S. S., and Hinshaw, S. P. (2006). Predictors of adolescent functioning in girls with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): the role of childhood ADHD, conduct problems, and peer status. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 35, 356–368. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3503-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loe, I. M., and Feldman, H. M. (2007). Academic and educational outcomes of children with ADHD. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 32, 643–654. doi: 10.1016/j.ambp.2006.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lundervold, A. J., Hinshaw, S., Sørensen, L., and Posserud, M. (2016). Co-occurring symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in a population-based sample of adolescents screened for depression. BMC Psychiatry 16:46. doi: 10.1186/s12888-016-0739-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mares, D., McLuckie, A., Schwartz, M., and Saini, M. (2007). Executive function impairments in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: do they differ between school and home environments? Can. J. Psychiatry 52, 527–534.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Miller, M., and Hinshaw, S. P. (2010). Does childhood executive function predict adolescent functional outcomes in girls with ADHD? J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 38, 315–326. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9369-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mitsis, E. M., McKay, K. E., Schulz, K. P., Newcorn, J. H., and Halperin, J. M. (2000). Parent–teacher concordance for DSM-IV attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a clinic-referred sample. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 39, 308–313. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200003000-00012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., and Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “Frontal Lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol. 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, D. W., Kollins, S. H., Hardy, K. K., Abikoff, H. B., Swanson, J. M., Cunningham, C., et al. (2007). Parent versus teacher ratings of attention- deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms in the Preschoolers with Attention- Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Treatment Study (PATS). J. Child Adolesc. Psychopharmacol. 17, 605–619. doi: 10.1089/cap.2007.0060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osler, M., Avlund, K., and Mortensen, E. L. (2013). Socio-economic position early in life, cognitive development and cognitive change from young adulthood to middle age. Eur. J. Public Health 23, 974–980. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/cks140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Owens, E. B., Zalecki, C., Gillette, P., and Hinshaw, S. P. (2017). Girls with childhood ADHD as adults: cross-domain outcomes by diagnostic persistence. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol. 85, 723–736. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Owens, J., and Jackson, H. (2017). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder severity, diagnosis, & later academic achievement in a national sample. Soc. Sci. Res. 61, 251–265. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2016.06.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pingault, J. B., Tremblay, R. E., Vitaro, F., Carbonneau, R., Genolini, C., Falissard, B., et al. (2011). Childhood trajectories of inattention and hyperactivity and prediction of educational attainment in early adulthood: a 16-year longitudinal population-based study. Am. J. Psychiatry 168, 1164–1170. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10121732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polderman, T. J. C., Boomsma, D. I., Bartels, M., Verhulst, F. C., and Huizink, A. C. (2010). A systematic review of prospective studies on attention problems and academic achievement. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 122, 271–284. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2010.01568.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polderman, T. J. C., Derks, E. M., Hudziak, J. J., Verhulst, F. C., Posthuma, D., and Boomsma, D. I. (2007). Across the continuum of attention skills: a twin study of the SWAN ADHD rating scale. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 48, 1080–1087. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10121732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rajendran, K., O’Neill, S., and Halperin, J. M. (2013). Inattention symptoms predict level of depression in early childhood. Postgrad. Med. 125, 154–161. doi: 10.3810/pgm.2013.01.2630

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rapport, M. D., Orban, S. A., Kofler, M. J., and Friedman, L. M. (2013). Do programs designed to train working memory, other executive functions, and attention benefit children with ADHD? A meta-analytic review of cognitive, academic, and behavioral outcomes. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 33, 1237–1252. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richardson, M., Abrahan, C., and Bond, R. (2012). Psychological correlates of university students’ academic performance: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 138, 353–387. doi: 10.1037/a0026838

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, M., Hwang, H., Toplak, M., Weiss, M., and Tannock, R. (2011). Inattention, working memory, and academic achievement in adolescents referred for attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Child Neuropsychol. 17, 444–458. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2010.544648

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A., Atkinson, T. M., and Berish, D. E. (2003). Executive functioning as a potential mediator of age-related cognitive decline in normal adults. J. Exp. Psychol. 132, 566–594. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.132.4.566

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaffer, D., Fisher, P., Lucas, C. P., Dulcan, M. K., and Schwab-Stone, M. E. (2000). NIMH diagnostic interview schedule for children version IV (NIMH DISC–IV): description, differences from previous versions, and reliability of some common diagnoses. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 39, 28–38. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200001000-00014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sonnander, S., Ramund, B., and Smedler, A. C. (1998). WISC-III Manual. Stockholm: Psykologiförlaget AB.

Google Scholar

Sonuga-Barke, E. J., Brandeis, D., Cortese, S., Daley, D., Ferrin, M., Holtmann, M., et al. (2013). Nonpharmacological interventions for ADHD: systematic review and meta-analyses of randomized controlled trials of dietary and psychological treatments. Am. J. Psychiatry 170, 275–289. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12070991

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2005). Causal models of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: from common simple deficits to multiple developmental pathways. Biol. Psychiatry 57, 1231–1238. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., and Halperin, J. M. (2010). Developmental phenotypes and causal pathways in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: potential targets for early intervention? J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 51, 368–389. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02195.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spencer, T. J., Biederman, J., and Mick, E. (2007). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: diagnosis, lifespan, comorbidities, and neurobiology. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 32, 631–642. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsm005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swanson, J. M. (1992). School-Based Assessments and Interventions for ADD Students. Kings Park, NY: KC publishing.

Google Scholar

Tamm, L., Epstein, J. N., Loren, R. E., Becker, S. P., Brenner, E. B., Bamberger, M. E., et al. (2017). Generating attention, inhibition, and memory: a pilot randomized trial for preschoolers with executive functioning deficits. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 20, 1–15. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2016.1266645

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Third Edition (WISC-III). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Wechsler, D. (1992). Wechsler Individual Achievement Test. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.

Google Scholar

Keywords: childhood inattention, academic achievement, culturally diverse, intellectual functioning, Bergen Child Study, Berkeley Girls with ADHD Longitudinal Study

Citation: Lundervold AJ, Meza JI, Hysing M and Hinshaw SP (2017) Parent Rated Symptoms of Inattention in Childhood Predict High School Academic Achievement Across Two Culturally and Diagnostically Diverse Samples. Front. Psychol. 8:1436. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01436

Received: 07 February 2017; Accepted: 08 August 2017;
Published: 25 August 2017.

Edited by:

Kai S. Cortina, University of Michigan, United States

Reviewed by:

Chris Lange-Küttner, London Metropolitan University, United Kingdom
Philip D. Zelazo, University of Minnesota, United States

Copyright © 2017 Lundervold, Meza, Hysing and Hinshaw. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Astri J. Lundervold, astri.lundervold@uib.no

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.