Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Psychol., 13 October 2017
Sec. Psychology of Language
This article is part of the Research Topic Approaches to Language: Data, Theory, and Explanation View all 21 articles

The Locus Preservation Hypothesis: Shared Linguistic Profiles across Developmental Disorders and the Resilient Part of the Human Language Faculty

  • 1Language and Culture, UiT-The Arctic University of Norway, Tromsø, Norway
  • 2Rehabilitation Sciences, Cyprus University of Technology, Limassol, Cyprus
  • 3Cyprus University of Technology, Limassol, Cyprus
  • 4English Studies, University of Cyprus, Nicosia, Cyprus

Grammatical markers are not uniformly impaired across speakers of different languages, even when speakers share a diagnosis and the marker in question is grammaticalized in a similar way in these languages. The aim of this work is to demarcate, from a cross-linguistic perspective, the linguistic phenotype of three genetically heterogeneous developmental disorders: specific language impairment, Down syndrome, and autism spectrum disorder. After a systematic review of linguistic profiles targeting mainly English-, Greek-, Catalan-, and Spanish-speaking populations with developmental disorders (n = 880), shared loci of impairment are identified and certain domains of grammar are shown to be more vulnerable than others. The distribution of impaired loci is captured by the Locus Preservation Hypothesis which suggests that specific parts of the language faculty are immune to impairment across developmental disorders. Through the Locus Preservation Hypothesis, a classical chicken and egg question can be addressed: Do poor conceptual resources and memory limitations result in an atypical grammar or does a grammatical breakdown lead to conceptual and memory limitations? Overall, certain morphological markers reveal themselves as highly susceptible to impairment, while syntactic operations are preserved, granting support to the first scenario. The origin of resilient syntax is explained from a phylogenetic perspective in connection to the “syntax-before-phonology” hypothesis.

Introduction

In his seminal book The Biological Foundations of Language, Eric Lenneberg made the following observation when comparing different states of verbal behavior:

Some aphasic symptoms bear certain similarities to the common derangements of speech and language seen in individuals in good health under conditions of mental exhaustion or states of drowsiness […]. Clinically, we may encounter an almost kaleidoscopic combination of idiosyncratic failure or sparing of particular skills which renders precise correlations between pathological anatomy and pathological verbal behavior very difficult (Lenneberg, 1967, p. 222; emphasis added).

Almost 40 years later, Phillips (2005) observed, when comparing the underpinnings of various developmental language impairments, that some aspects of language such as morphosyntactic difficulties associated with tense inflection appear to be affected across pathologies with different genetic causes (e.g., Specific Language Impairment, autism, Williams Syndrome, Down Syndrome, fragile X syndrome). Similarly, many studies have identified overlaps at the phenotypic level among different disorders: Leyfer et al. (2008) and Durrleman and Delage (2016) for autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and specific language impairment (SLI), Perovic et al. (2013) for ASD and Williams Syndrome, Dykens et al. (2011) for Prader-Willi syndrome and ASD, Eadie et al. (2002) for Down Syndrome (DS) and SLI, and Bishop (2014b) for anterior aphasia and SLI.

Overlaps in the behavioral profile of populations with different diagnoses have led to the claim that variation across phenotypes (i.e., breakdowns) is constrained in a way that renders some aspects of language processing—or more generally, cognition—more vulnerable in all pathological conditions, while others are consistently spared across individuals and conditions, both acquired and developmental (Phillips, 2005; Glisky, 2007; Kambanaros and van Steenbrugge, 2013; Benítez-Burraco and Boeckx, 2014; Leivada, 2014, 2015; Kambanaros and Grohmann, 2015; Tsimpli et al., 2017a). This high vulnerability of certain aspects of language is possibly the result of brain network organization. Studies on the distribution of lesions at the human connectome suggest that hubs are more likely to be anatomically abnormal than non-hubs across many, or possibly all, brain disorders because of their high centrality (van den Heuvel and Sporns, 2013; Crossley et al., 2014). Furthermore, multiple theoretical perspectives and neuroimaging research are addressing outstand ing questions about the nature and extent of brain connectivity aberrations in SLI vs. autism (Verhoeven et al., 2012) and DS (Anderson et al., 2013).

At the same time, studies disagree about the status of a grammatical marker as vulnerable or not, even when reporting on the competence and/or performance of speakers of the same language; for example, see Manika et al. (2010) for the greater variability that exists between studies that report on the status of clitics in Greek SLI. This phenotypic variability across linguistic profiles is observed even within one pedigree, where affected members share a diagnosis, as Bartha-Doering et al. (2016) have shown for SLI. One could, of course, argue that this is due to the character of SLI as a disorder that relies on an exclusionary diagnosis. In other words, because the criteria for diagnosing SLI are exclusionary (Reilly et al., 2014), this inevitably forms a largely heterogeneous disorder with diverse subtypes that encompass very different populations. However, the same phenotypic variability can be observed in impaired phenotypes that rely on an inclusionary diagnosis. For instance, Fowler (1995) notes that there is tremendous variability with respect to language function in individuals with DS. Lecavalier (2014) raises the same observation for ASD. Overall, this variability could be the result of variable expressivity. Individuals that carry a pathogenic variant of a gene can be impaired in a non-uniform fashion and this may result in different cognitive subtypes within an impaired phenotype (see Geschwind, 2011 for a review of variable expressivity in ASD). In this context, it becomes clear that the attained performance is not necessarily homogeneous even among people that share the same developmental disorder and speak the same language.

The picture painted by this brief overview involves a paradox. Although specific markers are highly vulnerable and as such prone to impairment across disorders, there still exists a lot of variability in terms of the attested impairment both across and within disorders. Phillips (2005) calls this state of affairs “a clear puzzle” and presents it in the following way:

On the one hand, the effects of specific genetic disorders on language appear to be surprisingly nonspecific. Similar aspects of language appear to be impacted across a variety of disorders with different genetic causes. On the other hand, the effects of genetic disorders on language are highly specific. [Developmental language impairments] appear to selectively target certain subparts of language while sparing others (Phillips, 2005, p. 79; emphases added).

This picture might even include derangements of speech in healthy, neurotypical adults, as noted in Lenneberg (1967) and quoted above.

In the present work, it is argued that the solution to Phillips' puzzle requires (i) a fine-grained analysis of loci of variation across different developmental impairments, which is (ii) situated within linguistic frameworks that put forth a clear division of labor between the different parts of grammar, and (iii) approached from a cross-linguistic perspective. In what follows we present work on (i), on (ii), and in parts on (iii), through comparing the linguistic profiles of three different types of developmental disorders (SLI, ASD, and DS) in speakers of two varieties of Greek (Stand ard Modern Greek and Cypriot Greek), English, Spanish, and Catalan.

We employ the layout of grammar put forth in the framework of Distributed Morphology (Halle and Marantz, 1993; Harley and Noyer, 1999; Figure 1) in order to identify which aspects of language feature the various loci of impairment. This model does not enhance the testability of our argument—but it does facilitate organizing the distribution of impaired markers across levels of linguistic analysis in a transparent way. In this framework (and minimalism at large), “syntactic derivation” refers to operations in syntax proper, the outcome of which feeds the other levels of analysis: phonology (via Phonetic/Phonological Form) and semantics (via Logical Form). Spell-out is an instruction to transfer this outcome to the next stage of operations.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Architecture of a minimalist/distributed morphology grammar (Bobaljik, 2017, p. 1).

By using a theoretical linguistics model as a vehicle for cartographing vulnerable loci across disorders, we establish an interdisciplinary connection between theoretical linguistics and the clinical aspects of cognitive neuroscience. Such interdisciplinary bridges are crucial in the study of language perhaps today more than ever, for it has been recently argued that linguistics, once seen the key player in the field of cognitive science, has seen its influence on closely allied disciplines fade away over the last years (Ferreira, 2005; Hagoort, 2014). However, one should not ignore the considerable body of literature that establishes interdisciplinary bridges in a way that shows how notions and primitives from theoretical linguistics can contribute to the study of neuroscience and other closely allied disciplines (Marantz, 2005; Sprouse and Almeida, 2013; Leivada, 2015). Against this background, the second aim of the present work is to offer a concrete example of how models of grammar in theoretical linguistics can inform the study of the brain through the investigation of pathological phenotypes. The study of the latter offers a unique perspective into the “physical mechanisms of the brain that correspond to the various domains of grammar and its structure” (Terzi, 2005, p. 111).

Methods

The case reports presented in the following are the result of extensive database searching through PubMed, SCOPUS, ScienceDirect, and Google Scholar, as well as probing individual journals for results retrieved by searches for any combination of the terms “primary/specific language impairment,” “autism spectrum disorder(s),” “Down('s) syndrome,” “linguistic phenotype,” “impaired/atypical phonology/morphology/syntax/semantics/pragmatics,” “word retrieval in SLI/ASD/DS,” and “linguistic impairment/disorder.” Our searches were constrained in terms of a time frame that covered the last two decades and in terms of language groups (Greek, English, Catalan, and Spanish). In choosing these language combinations, our aim was to cover both monolingual (Stand ard Modern Greek, English, Spanish) and bilectal/bilingual populations (Stand ard Modern Greek–Cypriot Greek, Spanish–Catalan) and languages with rich morphology. A cross-linguistic perspective is likely to shed light to the vulnerable parts of language in a way that goes beyond language-specific particularities. If any, it is the cross-linguistic study of the pathologies under investigation that has the potential to uncover the common denominator and the factors that distinguish children with a pathological linguistic profile from their typically developing peers (Leonard, 2014).

Specific Language Impairment (SLI)

Specific language impairment is a developmental disorder marked by limitations in the process of language development. It is usually assumed that these limitations occur in the absence of neurological damage such as hearing impairment, motor skills disorder, and low non-verbal IQ, and in the presence of otherwise typical cognitive development (Leonard, 1998). SLI is largely heterogeneous and many distinct subtypes have been identified in the literature. Two common SLI subtypes are typical SLI and pragmatic language impairment (Bishop, 2004): the former refers to those cases that involve problems with grammatical development (e.g., omission of paste tense morphemes in English), sometimes referred to as G(rammatical)-SLI (van der Lely, 2005) or Sy(ntactic)SLI (Friedmann and Novogrodsky, 2008), while the latter indicates social communication problems (e.g., lack of coherence in conversation). In some studies, these linguistic limitations have been grounded in cognition rather than language per se (e.g., working memory limitations; Gathercole and Baddeley, 1990; Dodwell and Bavin, 2008), leading to the conclusion that SLI is not really specific to language, as its name suggests (Engel de Abreu et al., 2014). This has led to serious debates, and no consensus, in the literature on terminology for defining SLI (Bishop, 2014a; Reilly et al., 2014). Table 1 presents 44 studies that feature different language groups and sets of tasks. These studies have been selected so as to include representation of all domains of impairment that have been proposed in the relevant literature on SLI.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. SLI studies across different language groups and sets of tasks.

Table 1 identifies all domains of grammar as potentially impaired in SLI populations. However, a closer look at the relevant results suggests that only some domains of grammar are truly atypical. It is clear that many studies report problems in morphophonology or pragmatics as well as general processing limitations. The nature of the impairment is less clear, though, in studies that argue in favor of a problem in the syntactic domain. Before showing why, we understand syntax as (the iterative application of) the operations (internal and external) Merge and Agree, following the definitions of Chomsky (2001). Many of the studies reviewed refer to omissions of agreement markers or failure to establish agreement/binding relations between different components of structure when talking about impaired syntax (e.g., Clahsen and Dalalakis, 1999; Tsimpli and Stavrakaki, 1999; Lin, 2007), and we follow the assumption that syntax indeed hosts these relations. The reason is that it is necessary to revisit the results of these studies—and explain in what sense they are not truly making a case for a deficient syntax—, instead of evoking an argument that dismisses the syntactic nature of these relations (binding/agreement) on theoretical grounds (e.g., by suggesting that Agree takes place post-syntactically, so when a study reports agreement errors, this does not concern syntax in the first place).

Returning to the studies in Table 1, Loeb et al. (1998) claim that the performance of the SLI group demonstrates a problem in syntax—yet their difference from controls is evident only in passives and some types of transitive–intransitive alternation responses but not in all. If the syntactic mechanisms responsible for this production were broken, how is it possible that they function for some types of stimuli? This variation suggests that these mechanisms are present and operative, but the overt realization of their output (“externalization”) might be affected depending on many factors such as the complexity of the task demand s (e.g., working memory overload).

Passivization is a classic example of the so-called syntactic deficit. As Penke (2015) notes, most language-impaired individuals would understand better a canonical SVO structure (e.g., “John kissed Mary”) compared to object clefts (e.g., “It is Mary who John kissed”) or passives (e.g., “Mary was kissed by John”). She notes that language-impaired individuals often misinterpret such structures by interpreting the first NP encountered as AGENT (as in the canonical SVO) instead of THEME. However, this is not a very concrete indication of a syntactic deficit for the following reason: The same mistake (i.e., the strategy to interpret the first NP of a clause as AGENT) is regularly observed in control groups that do not have any language impairment whatsoever (Penke, 2015). In other words, the same strategy is employed by healthy neurotypical subjects that have an intact, fully functional syntactic domain. This probably happens because the human parser establishes a threshold for the interpretation of each chunk of input. As noted in Leivada (2015), the strategy that Penke (2015) describes can be connected to the Moses illusion (Reder and Kusbit, 1991), according to which neurotypical individuals are unable to detect distortions in the experimental stimuli such as “How many animals of each kind did Moses take in the Ark?.” They might fail to detect the distortion even if they do know that it was Noah and not Moses who built the Ark. This phenomenon has been explained in the literature through recognizing the existence of a processing threshold by means of suggesting that a partial-match strategy is operative when the stimuli is processed (Kamas et al., 1996).

Pragmatic cues are very important when the parser establishes this threshold. For example, in relation to the Moses illusion, Moses and Noah are both biblical characters and as such loosely associated in a way that can trick the parser; if Nixon was used instead of Moses, it is much more likely that the distortion would be spotted (Kamas et al., 1996). Observing that all this happens in the case of neurotypical speakers, there is no reason not to capture the problems in passivization in (a)typical speakers in the same uniform way. It has been long noted that reversible passives (e.g., “The boy is being chased by the girl” and “The girl is being chased by the boy”) are more difficult to interpret compared to non-reversible passives which are at least pragmatically odd when reversed (e.g., “The task was carried out by John” and #“John was carried out by the task”), even in instances of typical language abilities (Rondal, 2007). Therefore, it comes as no surprise that many atypical populations show a selective impairment of passives: Reversible passives are impaired, while non-reversible passives are better preserved (see Caramazza and Miceli, 1991 for aphasia). In this context, it is somewhat expected that in atypical populations that have processing limitations (and many studies attest to this for SLI; see Table 1), lower accuracy will be observed in the comprehension of some passives—not because syntax is impaired, but because the partial-match process may operate at an overall lower threshold level perturbing comprehension.

Returning to studies that put forth a syntactic impairment, Marinis and van der Lely (2007) claim that children with SLI show a particular deficit in the computational system that affects syntactic dependencies involving syntactic movement: In contrast to controls, children with SLI showed no priming effect that would indicate a filler–gap dependency. At the same time, their very high performance (ca. 90% accuracy) suggests that they were somehow able to interpret the stimuli correctly. The priming effect that the results of Marinis and van der Lely (2007) showed at the verb position indicates that an association between two different syntactic positions was indeed established, which in turns means that the ability to form such associations remains operative in SLI populations. This begs the question: Which are then the factors that lead to what many studies describe as impaired or defective syntax?

On the basis of the studies presented in Table 1, we suggest that poor memory resources (Montgomery, 2004; Bishop and Donlan, 2005), Theory of Mind deficits (Tsimpli et al., 2017b), and spell-out errors (Lin, 2007; Mastropavlou and Tsimpli, 2011) can explain why a claim for impaired syntax is put forth. For example, Schuele and Dykes (2005) argue in their longitudinal study that certain aspects of syntax may be developed late. They report omissions of infinitival to, wh-pronouns in clausal interrogative complements, and relative markers. Importantly, this result is cross-linguistically supported (see Mastropavlou and Tsimpli, 2011 for omissions of such functional markers in Stand ard Modern Greek). Still, one cannot conclude that such omissions occur because these syntactic nodes are broken for two reasons. The first reason is that, even if a functional element is absent, its selectional requirements are fulfilled. The findings of Mastropavlou and Tsimpli (2011) show exactly this pattern:

This leads to a paradoxical situation where the complementizer may be omitted and, hence, not merged in the syntactic position, whereas its selectional restrictions are still operative. This is particularly relevant to the omission cases of na which, as mentioned above, is the only complementizer which can introduce tense-dependent verb forms, i.e., the non-past, perfective form. […] We must, therefore, conclude that even in the case of omissions, children know the selectional properties imposed by C and fail to access or spell-out the required complementizer” (Mastropavlou and Tsimpli, 2011, p. 460).

The second reason boils down to the fact that such omissions are never consistent; the markers in question are sometimes produced and sometimes omitted within a single speaker's productions. If we accept that these omissions are due to a retrieval problem at the level of externalization, we can explain the variation observed across productions as the result of any of the following factors as well as their possible interactions:

i. The presence or absence of salient pragmatic cues,

ii. Complexity and task-demand factors (that are related to memory limitations), and

iii. The (non-)salient morphophonological substance of the omitted markers (in line with the Surface Hypothesis, see Montgomery and Leonard, 1998).

If, however, the locus of impairment is the inability to construct a syntactic representation past a particular node, how is it possible that many times this syntactic representation is constructed and the problematic node surfaces intact? To give an example, if one suggests that the T(ense) node is problematic in Greek SLI, what explains that some affected persons might produce atypical realizations of T at times, while correctly producing T (and nodes past it) other times?

Having analyzed 682 linguistic profiles of children with SLI, we observe that the loci of impairment are related to externalization: morphophonology and pragmatics. Variation is attested in some parts of the language faculty and often appears in the form of omissions that occur due to retrieval/spell-out errors (Lin, 2007; Mastropavlou and Tsimpli, 2011), delayed mastery of phonology and failure to integrate related cues in overall processing (Kateri et al., 2005; Marshall et al., 2009), and processing, memory, and pragmatic limitations (Montgomery, 2004; Bishop and Donlan, 2005; Tsimpli et al., 2017b).

Comparing SLI with Other Disorders: ASD and DS

Pragmatic difficulties and morphophonological omissions are not restricted to SLI. The literature on ASD and DS has repeatedly highlighted the existence of such features in the linguistic profiles of these populations.

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD)

Starting off with ASD, the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5; American Psychiatric Association, 2013 p. 809) defines it as “characterized by deficits in two core domains: (1) deficits in social communication and social interaction and (2) restricted repetitive patterns of behavior, interests, and activities.”

The existence of repetitive patterns is one of the key characteristics of ASD language. Kanner (1943) was the first to describe instances of “parroting,” echolalia, and atypical use of personal pronouns that involved pronoun repetition in the autistic subjects of his study. Much subsequent work has focused on pronoun reversals (i.e., use of “you” instead of “I”) in ASD and many explanations have been offered for this phenomenon, including that of echolalia (Kanner's original explanation), impaired discourse understand ing, and impaired Theory of Mind (see Brehme, 2014 for a review). Some studies have described such reversals as grammatical errors, a description that may imply a deficient language module with impaired syntax (Bartolucci and Albers, 1974; Belkadi, 2006; Wittke et al., 2017).

Apart from pronoun reversals, some ASD linguistic profiles feature other types of grammatical errors (mainly in verbal, nominal, and pronominal morphology) in around 27–28% of their utterances, a result comparable to the frequency of grammatical errors in SLI (Wittke et al., 2017). Morphology stand s out as a vulnerable domain once more, and so do pragmatic abilities in ASD (Lord and Paul, 1997; Volden et al., 2009; Marinis et al., 2013).

Syntax in ASD has received mixed descriptions. On the one hand, Bartolucci and Albers (1974, p. 131) begin their study of tense marking in autism by postulating a syntactic problem: “Certain characteristics of the syntactic structures of the language of autistic children, such as their lack of mastery of pronominalization, have been described.” On the other hand, many reviews have concluded that ASD syntax is not deficient, since many syntactic dependencies remain intact especially in high-functioning individuals, but merely follow typical development at a slower rate (Tager-Flusberg, 1981; Perovic and Janke, 2013). Other studies revealed subtle difficulties in some syntactic measures, regardless of language development history (Durrleman et al., 2015). Looking at the relevant results, the notion of a processing threshold that was earlier discussed in relation to SLI becomes relevant for ASD too. For instance, the ASD group in Durrleman et al. (2015) obtained lower scores in the comprehension of object relative clauses compared to subject relative clauses. This asymmetry could boil down to the non-canonical word order derived by the fronted object in object relative clauses. In other words, this additional layer of complexity could be responsible for the subject–object asymmetry that is observed in the comprehension of relative clauses not only in ASD and SLI, but also in neurotypical populations, with subject relatives usually being easier to process (see Carreiras et al., 2010 for a review and a counterexample).

Returning to the atypical use of pronouns, Kanner (1943) and many subsequent studies indeed offer data that involve pronoun reversals. However, they also offer examples (of the same children, at the same stage of development) that show target use of pronouns (Leivada, 2015). If these pronoun reversals were the outcome of broken syntax, how is it possible that the target performance emerges at times? Put differently, if the locus of the deficiency is to be found in the innermost component of language (i.e., syntax), what makes possible the externalization of the target pattern often in a consistent fashion?

Interestingly, use of pronouns is not always atypical in ASD. Some studies have revealed high accuracy in the comprehension of different types of pronouns including strong pronouns, clitics, and reflexives (Terzi et al., 2012, 2014 for Stand ard Modern Greek). In these studies, the lowest performance was found in the clitics condition (mean correct: 88.3%) for which the most frequent error was theta-role reversals. Is this an indication of deficient syntax? As Terzi et al. (2012, 2014) show, these children had problems with producing clitic pronouns, so it is not clear whether their low performance in the clitics condition is the result of a problem in syntactic binding or the particular grammar of clitics. Terzi et al. (2014) carried out a follow-up study that aimed to clarify this issue. The results showed that the ASD group produced a high number of clitics, yet a lower one compared to the control group (87.39% correct vs. 97.74% correct, respectively), thus favoring the scenario that renders clitics and not binding responsible for the lower performance in the clitic condition of the task.

This lower performance of the ASD group in the clitics condition is compatible with the idea put forth in the present work that loci of impairment are confined to certain parts of the language faculty. We have argued that morphology and pragmatics are shown to be vulnerable across pathologies, languages, and elicitation tasks. Clitics are markers of morphological agreement, licensed under specific pragmatic conditions, and children with ASD have troubles in ascertaining what is prominent/salient in the discourse (Terzi et al., 2016). In a subsequent study that involved narratives instead of a highly structured elicitation task, Terzi et al. (2017) found that the same group of ASD children did produce clitics, a fact that highlights the importance of the tool used to elicit data. According to Terzi et al. (2017, p. 648), ASD children “had full control of the discourse by contrast to the structured experiments, the nature of which was such that they had to take into account the discourse representation provided by the experimenter in each condition and trial.”

In this context, it could well be the case that pronoun reversals in ASD do not stem from impaired language/syntax. Studies of deaf children with autism provided below lend support for this hypothesis. It has been suggested in the relevant literature that what seems to be at stake in ASD is a less secure anchorage in self-experience (Lee et al., 1994). Shield and Meier's (2014) experiment is instrumental in evaluating this hypothesis. They showed deaf autistic and deaf typically developing children a picture of themselves and a picture of the experimenter. Upon seeing a picture of themselves and being asked “Who is this?,” the children with ASD either signed the pronoun “me” pointing to themselves or produced their name sign or finger-spelled their English name. In other words, they were successful both in identifying themselves and in using the correct pronoun, whenever a pronoun was used. The same strategies were employed by the typically developing group. What differentiated the two groups is not self-identification per se or the linguistic strategy through which self-identification was achieved, but the fact that the typically developing children “reacted with a smile or laugh and an emphatic point at his/her own body. The children with ASD had no such emotional reaction.” (Shield and Meier, 2014, p. 412). As the authors note in their discussion of these findings, forming a sense of me-ness is a key component of social behaviors such as empathy.

This less secure anchorage in me-ness can be manifested in ways that have nothing to do with the use of pronouns, thereby suggesting that the pronoun-reversal problem is not linguistic or syntactic as such (Leivada, 2015). A crucial piece of evidence that leads to this conclusion comes from studies of palm orientation during signing by deaf children with ASD. Shield and Meier (2012) found that native signers of American Sign Language with ASD showed a tendency to reverse palm orientation on signs specified for inward/outward orientation, whereas such errors were absent from the production of their typically developing peers. Observing this atypical anchorage in selfhood, one can suggest that their linguistic/grammatical counterparts (i.e., pronoun reversals) reflect not a syntactic problem but rather a more general cognitive problem that may acquire a linguistic dress (Leivada, 2015). If this observation is on the right track, syntax seems to be unimpaired in ASD, whereas other domains of language such as morphophonology (Kanner, 1943) and pragmatics (Terzi et al., 2014) stand out as particularly susceptible to impairment.

Down Syndrome (DS)

DS is the result of a genetic abnormality most often caused from the presence of a third chromosome 21. One of the characteristics of this syndrome is atypical cognitive development. When it comes to language, our review of studies on DS suggest it is somewhat challenging when one pursues a claim of preserved syntax (as some studies have identified syntactic deficits in the profile of their subjects; e.g., Perovic, 2001).

One domain of language that has been argued to be atypical in DS is syntactic binding. Binding Theory regulates the distribution of referentially dependent elements such as anaphors and pronouns (Chomsky, 1981). Binding Principle A requires that the anaphor is locally bound by an antecedent within the same clause/domain (e.g., Maryi criticized herselfi/*j). Principle B requires that the antecedent of a pronoun be not in the same clause/domain as the pronoun (e.g., Billj said that Johni criticized himj/*i). Principle C prohibits a referential expression from being c-command ed by a coindexed element (e.g., Hei/Billi criticized Johnj/*i).

Investigating the comprehension abilities of English-speaking adolescents with DS using a truth-value judgment task, Perovic (2001) found at ceiling performance on the “name-pronoun” condition (e.g., “Is Snow White washing her?”) and high performance (≥75%) for the “quantifier-pronoun” condition (e.g., “Is every bear washing him?”). This suggests that whatever the syntactic deficit amounts to, it is not Principle B. The conditions “name-reflexive” (e.g., “Is Snow White washing herself?”) and “quantifier-reflexive” (e.g., “Is every bear washing himself?”) elicited mixed responses with the percentage of correct answers ranging from 12.50 to 100% correct.

Is Principle A an example of deficient syntax in DS? The answer must be negative for a number of reasons (see Leivada, 2015 for more extensive discussion). First, would be a non-trivial task to explain why individuals with a deficient syntax would face difficulties with one binding principle but not with another, given that all binding principles require the same underlying grammatical knowledge (Perovic, 2001). Second, the results did not show a unanimous pattern of Principle A violations. The average number of correct responses on the “name-reflexive” condition was above chance (56.56% correct). In turn, the average number of correct responses on the “quantifier-reflexive” condition was below chance (35.94% correct), but as Perovic (2001) noted, two participants showed very poor performance even on the control condition that involved quantified NPs and no anaphors. It is then possible that these participants had issues with quantification generally, which resulted in errors on some of the tested conditions.

Tsakiridou (2006) and Christodoulou (2011) focused on Standard Modern Greek and Cypriot Greek DS grammars, respectively. Both showed that the deviations noted in the DS linguistic profile were related to morphophonology: non-target morphological markings (Tsakiridou, 2006) and phonetically or morphophonologically conditioned differences (Christodoulou, 2011). Pragmatics in DS is also atypical. Challenges may include initiation of topics and communicative repairs and aspects of narratives (Martin et al., 2009).

The overall picture that emerges with respect to the linguistic phenotype of DS is one that supports the claim that the aspects of language which appear to be atypical are related to specific parts of the language faculty: morphophonology and pragmatics.

The Locus Preservation Hypothesis

Having reviewed the literature on three developmental disorders, the first observation is that certain morphological markers reveal themselves as highly susceptible to impairment (e.g., agreement markers and clitics). Second, syntax appears to be preserved. Undoubtedly, some studies have identified problems in the comprehension or production of complex syntactic structures across disorders (see Table 1 for SLI). Yet, when considering the general processing limitations that are arguably present in the pathologies discussed in the present work (even though, unfortunately, not fully or equally measured in all studies), we are facing a classical chicken and egg question (Bishop and Donlan, 2005): Do syntactic limitations lead to conceptual and memory limitations or do conceptual and memory limitations result in an atypical syntax?

We have argued that poor memory resources (Montgomery, 2004; Bishop and Donlan, 2005), Theory of Mind deficits (Tsimpli et al., 2017b), and retrieval/spell-out errors (Lin, 2007; Mastropavlou and Tsimpli, 2011) can explain why a claim for impaired syntax is put forth at times. Observing how “linguistic” deficits such as the incorrect use of anaphors in ASD can derive from a general cognitive problem in establishing me-ness in relation to the outer world, we tentatively conclude that atypical cognitive abilities (i.e., processing impairments, memory limitations; see Table 1) may result in what looks as an atypical syntax. The latter is manifested mainly through omissions, and recall that it would be wrong to conclude that such omissions occur because the related syntactic nodes or operations are broken. The selectional requirements of omitted functional elements may still be operative and satisfied (Mastropavlou and Tsimpli, 2011). Therefore, it makes more sense to describe such omissions as spell-out errors related to the externalization component of language.

Looking at the distribution of impaired and preserved markers/levels of linguistic analysis, variation across pathologies can by formally captured within the Locus Preservation Hypothesis (see also Leivada, 2015 for an earlier formulation based on Greek data only):

(1) Locus Preservation Hypothesis

Syntactic operations are preserved and impenetrable to variation across developmental pathologies.

Assuming a widely accepted architecture of the grammar as the one shown in Figure 2, the Locus Preservation Hypothesis holds that the computational part of the human language faculty is invariably preserved, with the operations (internal and external) Merge and Agree applying in an intact manner all the way to constructing the internal interface levels of Logical Form (LF) and Phonetic/Phonological Form (PF).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. A minimalist architecture of the grammar (Tsimpli et al., 2017a, p. 494).

The purported pragmatic deficiencies (Katsos et al., 2011) arise post-syntactically, where the conceptual-intentional system (CI) is accessed along with pragmatic information and encyclopedic/world knowledge. Likewise, the externalization difficulty observed in language production tasks and spontaneous speech (Mastropavlou and Tsimpli, 2011) is relevant at the other interface, the articulatory-perceptual or sensory-motor system (SM). The fact that bound morphophonological building blocks are often misused (Bedore and Leonard, 2001) suggests the need for a finer distinction of the “Lexicon” than what the architecture in Figure 2 allows.

Mapping the Locus Preservation Hypothesis to the distribution of labor put forth in Distributed Morphology, it seems that the first set of operations in the transition from List A to List B are resilient to impairment across atypical cognitive phenotypes. In contrast, morphophonological operations and encyclopedic knowledge are consistently susceptible to impairment across atypical cognitive phenotypes (Leivada, 2015). The results that led to this conclusion come from three developmental disorders (SLI, ASD, DS), but there are reasons to believe that this conclusion would hold even when one examines the linguistic profile of acquired pathologies such as aphasia; a topic to be pursed in future work on the Locus Preservation Hypothesis. A more detailed model is provided in Figure 3.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Distribution of labor in distributed morphology (Harley and Noyer, 1999, p. 3).

Overall, based on our review of different research studies, not all pathologies show the same impaired markers—but the same markers are consistently impaired across pathologies. The Locus Preservation Hypothesis is thus pathology-independent and can be used to support cross-linguistic findings.

The important question is why syntactic operations are better preserved in a consistent way across disorders with different genetic etiology. One explanation is that the phenotypic overlaps that we identified are in fact surface reflections of more deeply rooted overlaps at the connectome or even the oscillome (Benítez-Burraco and Murphy, 2016). Observing that the hierarchy of brain oscillations has remained remarkably preserved during mammalian evolution (Buzsáki et al., 2013), Benítez-Burraco and Murphy (2016) suggest that language deficits in various cognitive disorders can be traced back to a brain syntax network. In this context, it can be argued that syntax is preserved because it is implemented through a network that is less novel in evolutionary terms, hence more resilient to impairment. Less resilient networks underlie cognitive capacities more recently evolved in phylogenetic terms, whereby selective pressures have not yet given rise to the development of robust compensatory mechanisms (Toro et al., 2010; Murphy and Benítez-Burraco, 2016). This claim grants support to another hypothesis recently explored in the language evolution literature: the “syntax-before-phonology” hypothesis. Based on a review of linguistic calls across species, Collier et al. (2014) argue that syntax, which is universally present in all languages, possibly evolved before phonology, since many systems of communication in other species have the former but not the latter. The Locus Preservation Hypothesis suggests that phonology is less resilient in stark contrast to syntax—a finding that is in line with what the ethological record reveals (Collier et al., 2014).

Conclusions

The present work has put forth a novel hypothesis: the Locus Preservation Hypothesis, in order to capture the distribution of what are considered atypical linguistic markers across different languages and pathologies. It has been argued that syntactic operations are resilient to impairment across developmental disorders; in contrast, morphophonology and pragmatics are consistently impaired. This conclusion stand s in agreement with a long line of literature that discusses overlaps in the behavioral profile of populations with different pathologies, both acquired and developmental (Phillips, 2005; Glisky, 2007; Kambanaros and van Steenbrugge, 2013; Benítez-Burraco and Boeckx, 2014; Leivada, 2014, 2015; Kambanaros and Grohmann, 2015; Tsimpli et al., 2017a).

The Locus Preservation Hypothesis can gain more support by expanding the range of languages and pathologies that are examined. Once this is done, the following question to be explored in detail is why syntax would be preserved. One explanation we contemplated in the present work relates to the possibility of an underlying uniform etiology across the reviewed disorders. This uniformity can be traced back to brain network organization (van den Heuvel and Sporns, 2013; Crossley et al., 2014; Benítez-Burraco and Murphy, 2016). Addressing the parallels that can be observed across different levels of representation (phenome, connectome, dynome, and oscillome) from a phylogenetic perspective, we have established a connection between the hypothesis put forth in the present work and the “syntax-before-phonology” hypothesis of Collier et al. (2014): Syntax is better preserved because it evolved before other domains of language (e.g., morphology and phonology). Therefore, syntax had more adaptation time for the development of compensatory mechanisms, unlike more recently evolved cognitive/linguistic capacities. Future research on the Locus Preservation Hypothesis will elaborate on the syntax-first hypothesis and flesh out the connections between the observed overlap at the phenotypic level and its roots in deeper levels of representation.

Author Contributions

All authors participated in the analysis. EL drafted the manuscript. MK and KG reviewed and revised the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank the two reviewers and Nikoleta Christou for helpful comments. We also thank the audiences of the conferences where this work was presented in Vienna, Patras, and Barcelona. The publication charges for this article have been funded by a grant from the publication fund of UiT The Arctic University of Norway.

References

Aguilar-Mediavilla, E., Buil-Legaz, L., Pérez-Castelló, J. A., Rigo-Carratalà, E., and Adrover-Roig, D. (2014). Early preschool processing abilities predict subsequent reading outcomes in bilingual Spanish–Catalan children with specific language impairment (SLI). J. Commun. Disord. 50, 19–35. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2014.03.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aguilar-Mediavilla, E., and Serra-Raventós, M. (2006). Phonological profile of Spanish–Catalan children with specific language impairment at age 4: are there any changes over time? Folia Phoniatr. Logoped. 58, 400–414. doi: 10.1159/000095001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders—(DSM-V). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

Anderson, J. S., Nielsen, J. A., Ferguson, M. A., Burback, M. C., Cox, E. T., Dai, L., et al. (2013). Abnormal brain synchrony in Down Syndrome. Neuroimage 24, 703–715. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2013.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andreu, L., Sanz-Torrent, M., Olmos, J. G., and MacWhinney, B. (2013). The formulation of argument structure in SLI: an eye-movement study. Clin. Linguist. Phonet. 27, 111–133. doi: 10.3109/02699206.2012.751623

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Andreu, L., Sanz-Torrent, M., and Rodríguez-Ferreiro, J. (2016). Do children with SLI use verbs to predict arguments and adjuncts: evidence from eye movements during listening. Front. Psychol. 6:1917. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01917

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartha-Doering, L., Regele, S., Deuster, D., Seidl, R., Bogdanova, N., Röpke, A., et al. (2016). Intrafamilial phenotypic variability of specific language impairment. Brain Lang. 159, 102–108. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2016.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bartolucci, G., and Albers, R. J. (1974). Deictic categories in the language of autistic children. J. Autism Child. Schizophr. 4, 131–141. doi: 10.1007/BF02105366

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bedore, L. M., and Leonard, L. B. (2001). Grammatical morphology deficits in Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 44, 905–924. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2001/072)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belkadi, A. (2006). Language impairments in autism: evidence against mind-blindness. SOAS Work. Papers Ling. 14, 3–13.

Google Scholar

Benítez-Burraco, A., and Boeckx, C. (2014). Universal Grammar and biological variation: an EvoDevo agenda for comparative biolinguistics. Biol. Theory 9, 122–134. doi: 10.1007/s13752-014-0164-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benítez-Burraco, A., and Murphy, E. (2016). The oscillopathic nature of language deficits in autism: from genes to language evolution. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:120. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bishop, D. V. M. (2004). “Specific Language Impairment: diagnostic dilemmas,” in Classification of Developmental Language Disorders, eds L. Verhoeven and H. van Balkom (Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum), 309–326.

Google Scholar

Bishop, D. V. (2014a). Ten questions about terminology for children with unexplained language problems. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 49, 381–415. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bishop, D. V. (2014b). Problems with tense marking in children with specific language impairment: not how but when. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 369:20120401. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2012.0401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bishop, D. V. M., and Donlan, C. (2005). The role of syntax in encoding and recall of pictorial narratives: evidence from specific language impairment. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 23, 25–46. doi: 10.1348/026151004X20685

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bobaljik, J. D. (2017). Distributed morphology. Oxford Res. Encyclop. Linguist. doi: 10.1093/acrefore/9780199384655.013.131

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brehme, D. F. (2014). Perspectives on personal pronoun reversal in children with ASD: a critical review. J. Euro. Psychol. Stud. 5, 31–37. doi: 10.5334/jeps.br

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buil-Legaz, L., Pérez-Castelló, J. A., Adrover-Roig, D., and Aguilar-Mediavilla, E. (2016). Referential communication in bilingual Catalan–Spanish children with specific language impairment. Anuar. Psicol. 46, 31–40. doi: 10.1016/j.anpsic.2016.06.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsáki, G., Logothetis, N., and Singer, W. (2013). Scaling brain size, keeping timing: evolutionary preservation of brain rhythms. Neuron 80, 751–764. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caramazza, A., and Miceli, G. (1991). Selective impairment of thematic role assignment in sentence processing. Brain Lang. 41, 402–436. doi: 10.1016/0093-934X(91)90164-V

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carreiras, M., Duñabeitia, J. A., Vergara, M., de la Cruz-Pavía, I., and Laka, I. (2010). Subject relative clauses are not universally easier to process: evidence from Basque. Cognition 115, 79–92. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.11.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chomsky, N. (1981). Lectures on Government and Binding. Dordrecht: Foris.

Google Scholar

Chomsky, N. (2001). “Derivation by phase,” in Ken Hale: A Life in Language, ed M. Kenstowicz (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 1–52.

Christodoulou, C. (2011). Cypriot Greek Down Syndrome: Their Grammar and Its Interfaces. Doctoral Dissertation, The University of British Columbia.

Google Scholar

Claessen, M., and Leitão, S. (2012). Phonological representations in children with SLI. Child Lang. Teach. Ther. 28, 211–223. doi: 10.1177/0265659012436851

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clahsen, H., and Dalalakis, J. (1999). Tense and agreement in Greek SLI: a case study. Essex Res. Rep. Linguist. 24, 1–25.

Google Scholar

Collier, K., Bickel, B., van Schaik, C. P., Manser, M. B., and Townsend, S. W. (2014). Language evolution: syntax before phonology? Proc. R. Soc. B 281:20140263. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2014.0263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crossley, N. A., Mechelli, A., Scott, J., Carletti, F., Fox, P. T., McGuire, P., et al. (2014). The hubs of the human connectome are generally implicated in the anatomy of brain disorders. Brain 137, 2382–2395. doi: 10.1093/brain/awu132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dodwell, K., and Bavin, E. L. (2008). Children with specific language impairment: an investigation of their narratives and memory. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 43, 201–218. doi: 10.1080/13682820701366147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durrleman, S., and Delage, H. (2016). Autism spectrum disorder and specific language impairment: overlaps in syntactic profiles. Lang. Acquist. 23, 361–386. doi: 10.1080/10489223.2016.1179741

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Durrleman, S., Hippolyte, L., Zufferey, S., Iglesias, K., and Hadjikhani, N. (2015). Complex syntax in autism spectrum disorders: a study of relative clauses. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 50, 260–267. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dykens, E. M., Lee, E., and Roof, E. (2011). Prader-Willi syndrome and autism spectrum disorders: an evolving story. J. Neurodev. Disord. 3, 225–237. doi: 10.1007/s11689-011-9092-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eadie, P. A., Fey, M. E., Douglas, J. M., and Parsons, C. L. (2002). Profiles of grammatical morphology and sentence imitation in children with specific language impairment and Down syndrome. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 45, 720–732. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2002/058)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engel de Abreu, P. M., Cruz-Santos, A., and Puglisi, M. L. (2014). Specific language impairment in language-minority children from low-income families. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 49, 736–747. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ferreira, F. (2005). Psycholinguistics, formal grammars, and cognitive science. Linguist. Rev. 22, 365–380. doi: 10.1515/tlir.2005.22.2-4.365

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fowler, A. E. (1995). “Linguistic variability in persons with down syndrome: research and implications,” in Down Syndrome: Living and Learning in the Community, eds L. Nadel and D. Rosenthal (New York, NY: Wiley-Liss), 121–131.

Google Scholar

Friedmann, N., and Novogrodsky, R. (2008). “Subtypes of SLI: SySLI, PhoSLI, LeSLI, and PraSLIm,” in Language Acquisition and Development: Proceedings of GALA 2007, eds A. Gavarró and M. J. Freitas (Newcastle: Cambridge Scholars Publishing), 205–217.

Google Scholar

Gathercole, S., and Baddeley, A. (1990). Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: is there a causal connection? J. Mem. Lang. 29, 336–360. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(90)90004-J

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gavarró, A., and Cantú-Sánchez, M. (2016). “A study on an alleged case of Spanish SLI and the founder effect,” in Biolinguistic Investigations on the Language Faculty, ed A. M. Di Sciullo (Amsterdam: John Benjamins), 169–193.

Google Scholar

Gavarró, A., and Lite, A. (2015). “Universal quantification in Catalan SLI,” in Specific Language Impairment: Current Trends in Research, ed S. Stavrakaki (Amsterdam: John Benjamins), 191–213.

Google Scholar

Geschwind, D. H. (2011). Genetics of autism spectrum disorders. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 409–416. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Glisky, E. L. (2007). “Changes in cognitive function in human aging,” in Brain Aging: Models, Methods, and Mechanisms, ed D. R. Riddle (Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press), 4–20.

Grinstead, J., Baron, A., Vega-Mendoza, M., De la Mora, J., Cantú-Sánchez, M., and Flores, B. (2013). Tense marking and spontaneous speech measures in Spanish specific language impairment: a discriminant function analysis. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 56, 352–363. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0289)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagoort, P. (2014). “Linguistics quo vadis? An outsider perspective,” in Talk Presented at SLE 2014: 47th Annual Meeting of the Societas Linguistica Europaea (Poznan: Adam Mickiewicz University). Available online at: http://thescienceoflanguage.com/2015/02/16/linguistics-quo-vadis-an-outsider-perspective

Halle, M., and Marantz, A. (1993). “Distributed morphology and the pieces of inflection,” in The View from Building 20: Essays in Linguistics in Honor of Sylvain Bromberger, eds K. Hale and S. J. Keyser (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 111–176.

Google Scholar

Harley, H., and Noyer, R. (1999). Distributed morphology. Glot. Int. 4, 3–9.

Google Scholar

Kamas, E. N., Reder, L. M., and Ayers, M. S. (1996). Partial matching in the Moses illusion: response bias not sensitivity. Mem. Cogn. 24, 687–699. doi: 10.3758/BF03201094

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kambanaros, M. (2013). Does verb type affect action naming in specific language impairment (SLI)? Evidence from instrumentality and name relation. J. Neurolinguist. 26, 160–177. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroling.2012.07.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kambanaros, M. (2014). Context effects on verb production in specific language impairment (SLI): confrontation naming versus connected speech. Clin. Linguist. Phonet. 28, 826–843. doi: 10.3109/02699206.2014.911962

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kambanaros, M., and Grohmann, K. K. (2015). Grammatical class effects across impaired child and adult populations. Front. Psychol. Lang. Sci. 6:1670. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kambanaros, M., Grohmann, K. K., Michaelides, M., and Theodorou, E. (2014). On the nature of verb–noun dissociations in bilectal SLI: a psycholinguistic perspective from Greek. Bilingualism 17, 169–188. doi: 10.1017/S1366728913000035

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kambanaros, M., and van Steenbrugge, W. (2013). Lexical retrieval deficits in anomic aphasia and specific language impairment (SLI): more similar than different? Grammatical class and context effects. Linguist. Variat. 13, 237–256. doi: 10.1075/lv.13.2.05kam

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nerv. Child 2, 217–250.

Google Scholar

Kateri, L., Revithiadou, A., and Varlokosta, S. (2005). “Multiple partially ordered grammars: a case study from Greek SLI,” in Developmental Paths in Phonological Acquisition, eds M. Tzakosta, C. Levelt and J. van de Weijer (Leiden: Leiden Centre for Linguistics), 13–38.

Google Scholar

Katsos, N., Roqueta, C. A., Estevan, R. A., and Cummins, C. (2011). Are children with specific language impairment competent with the pragmatics and logic of quantification? Cognition 119, 43–57. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2010.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lalioti, M., Stavrakaki, S., Manouilidou, C., and Talli, I. (2016). Subject–verb agreement and verbal short-term memory: a perspective from Greek children with specific language impairment. First Language 36, 279–294. doi: 10.1177/0142723716648844

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lecavalier, L. (2014). “Phenotypic variability in autism spectrum disorder: clinical considerations,” in Hand book of Autism and Anxiety, eds T. E. Davis III, S. W. White and T. H. Ollendick (Cham: Springer International Publishing), 15–29.

Google Scholar

Lee, A., Hobson, R. P., and Chiat, S. (1994). I, you, me, and autism: an experimental study. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 24, 155–176. doi: 10.1007/BF02172094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leivada, E. (2014). From comparative languistics to comparative (bio)linguistics: reflections on variation. Biolinguistics 8, 53–66.

Google Scholar

Leivada, E. (2015). The Nature and Limits of Variation across Languages and Pathologies. Doctoral Dissertation, Universitat de Barcelona.

Google Scholar

Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). Biological Foundations of Language. New York, NY: John Wiley and Sons.

Google Scholar

Leonard, L. B. (1998). Children with Specific Language Impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Google Scholar

Leonard, L. B. (2014). Specific language impairment across languages. Child Dev. Perspect. 8, 1–5. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leonard, L. B., Deevy, P., Fey, M. E., and Bredin-Oja, S. L. (2013). Sentence comprehension in specific language impairment: a task designed to distinguish between cognitive capacity and syntactic complexity. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 56, 577–589. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2012/11-0254)

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leonard, L. B., Deevy, P., Miller, C. A., Charest, M., Kurtz, R., and Rauf, L. (2003). The use of grammatical morphemes reflecting aspect and modality by children with specific language impairment. J. Child Lang. 30, 769–795. doi: 10.1017/S0305000903005816

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leyfer, O. T., Tager-Flusberg, H., Dowd, M., Tomblin, J. B., and Folstein, S. E. (2008). Overlap between autism and specific language impairment: comparison of autism diagnostic interview and autism diagnostic observation schedule scores. Autism Res. 1, 284–296. doi: 10.1002/aur.43

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lin, Y.-A. (2007). “On grammatical errors in English SLI children: a corpus-based study,” in Proceedings of the 2nd Conference on Generative Approaches to Language Acquisition North America (GALANA), eds A. Belikova, L. Meroni and M. Umeda (Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings Project), 245–252.

Google Scholar

Loeb, D. F., Pye, C., Richardson, L. Z., and Redmond, S. (1998). Causative alternations of children with specific language impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 41, 1103–1114. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4105.110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lord, C., and Paul, R. (1997). “Language and communication in autism,” in Hand book of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders, 2nd Edn, eds F. Volkmar and D. Cohen (New York, NY: John Wiley and Sons), 195–225.

Manika, S., Varlokosta, S., and Wexler, K. (2010). “The lack of omission of clitics in Greek children with SLI: an experimental study,” in Proceedings of the 35th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, Vol. 35, 427–439.

Google Scholar

Marantz, A. (2005). Generative linguistics within the cognitive neuroscience of language. Linguist. Rev. 22, 429–445. doi: 10.1515/tlir.2005.22.2-4.429

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marinis, T., and Saddy, D. (2013). Parsing the passive: comparing children with specific language impairment to sequential bilingual children. Lang. Acquist. 20, 155–179. doi: 10.1080/10489223.2013.766743

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marinis, T., Terzi, A., Kotsopoulou, A., and Francis, K. (2013). Pragmatic abilities of high-functioning Greek-speaking children with autism. Psychologia 20, 321–337.

Google Scholar

Marinis, T., and van der Lely, H. K. (2007). On-line processing of wh-questions in children with G-SLI and typically developing children. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 42, 557–582. doi: 10.1080/13682820601058190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marshall, C. R., Harcourt-Brown, S., Ramus, F., and van der Lely, H. K. J. (2009). The link between prosody and language skills in children with specific language impairment (SLI) and /or dyslexia. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 44, 466–488. doi: 10.1080/13682820802591643

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martin, G. E., Klusek, J., Estigarribia, B., and Roberts, J. E. (2009). Language characteristics of individuals with Down syndrome. Top. Lang. Disord. 29, 112–132.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Mastropavlou, M. (2010). Morphophonological salience as a compensatory means for deficits in the acquisition of past tense in SLI. J. Commun. Disord. 43, 175–198. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2009.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mastropavlou, M., and Tsimpli, I. M. (2011). Complementizers and subordination in typical language acquisition and SLI. Lingua 121, 442–462. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2010.10.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montgomery, J. W. (2004). Sentence comprehension in children with specific language impairment: effects of input rate and phonological working memory. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 39, 115–133. doi: 10.1080/13682820310001616985

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montgomery, J. W., and Leonard, L. B. (1998). Real-time inflectional processing by children with specific language impairment: effects of phonetic substance. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 41, 1432–1443. doi: 10.1044/jslhr.4106.1432

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, E., and Benítez-Burraco, A. (2016). Bridging the gap between genes and language deficits in schizophrenia: an oscillopathic approach. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:422. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O'Hara, M., and Johnston, J. (1997). Syntactic bootstrapping in children with specific language impairment. Euro. J. Disord. Commun. 32, 189–205.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Owen Van Horne, A. J., and Lin, S. (2011). Cognitive state verbs and complement clauses in children with SLI and their typically developing peers. Clin. Linguist. Phonet. 25, 881–898. doi: 10.3109/02699206.2011.582226

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penke, M. (2015). “Syntax and language disorders,” in Syntax — An International Hand book of Contemporary Research, ed A. Alexiadou and T. Kiss (Berlin: Walter de Gruyter), 1833–1874.

Google Scholar

Perovic, A. (2001). Binding principles in Down syndrome. UCL Work. Pap. Linguist. 13, 423–455.

Google Scholar

Perovic, A., and Janke, V. (2013). Issues in the acquisition of binding, control and raising in high-functioning children with autism. UCL Work. Pap. Linguist. 25, 131–143.

Google Scholar

Perovic, A., Modyanova, N., and Wexler, K. (2013). Comparison of grammar in neurodevelopmental disorders: the case of binding in Williams syndrome and autism with and without language impairment. Lang. Acquist. 20, 133–154. doi: 10.1080/10489223.2013.766742

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petinou, K., and Terzi, A. (2002). Clitic misplacement among normally developing children and children with specific language impairment and the status of Infl heads. Lang. Acquis. 10, 1–28. doi: 10.1207/S15327817LA1001_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phillips, C. (2005). Electrophysiology in the study of developmental language impairments: prospects and challenges for a top-down approach. Appl. Psycholinguist. 26, 79–96. doi: 10.1017/S0142716405050071

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reder, L. M., and Kusbit, G. W. (1991). Locus of the Moses illusion: imperfect encoding, retrieval, or match? J. Mem. Lang. 30, 385–406. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(91)90013-A

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reilly, S., Tomblin, B., Law, J., McKean, C., Mensah, F. K., Morgan, A., et al. (2014). Specific language impairment: a convenient label for whom? Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 49, 416–451. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Restrepo, M. A., and Gutierrez-Clellen, V. F. (2001). Article use in Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. J. Child Lang. 28, 433–452. doi: 10.1017/S0305000901004706

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riches, N. G. (2012). Sentence repetition in children with specific language impairment: an investigation of underlying mechanisms. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 47, 499–510. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-6984.2012.00158.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riches, N. G., Loucas, T., Baird, G., Charman, T., and Simonoff, E. (2010). Sentence repetition in adolescents with specific language impairments and autism: an investigation of complex syntax. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 45, 47–60. doi: 10.3109/13682820802647676

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rondal, J.-A. (2007). “Language rehabilitation,” in Therapies and Rehabilitation in Down Syndrome, eds J.-A. Rondal and A. Rasore Quartino (London: John Wiley and Sons), 63–89.

Google Scholar

Sanz-Torrent, M., Serrat, E., Andreu, L., and Serra, M. (2008). Verb morphology in Catalan and Spanish in children with specific language impairment: a developmental study. Clin. Linguist. Phonet. 22, 459–474. doi: 10.1080/02699200801892959

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schuele, C. M., and Dykes, J. C. (2005). Complex syntax acquisition: a longitudinal case study of a child with specific language impairment. Clin. Linguist. Phonet. 19, 295–318. doi: 10.1080/02699200410001703709

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shield, A., and Meier, R. P. (2012). Palm reversal errors in native-signing children with autism. J. Commun. Disord. 45, 439–454. doi: 10.1016/j.jcomdis.2012.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shield, A., and Meier, R. P. (2014). Personal pronoun avoidance in deaf children with autism. Proc. BUCLD 38, 403–415.

Google Scholar

Spanoudis, G., Natsopoulos, D., and Panayiotou, G. (2007). Mental verbs and pragmatic language difficulties. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 42, 487–504. doi: 10.1080/13682820601010027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sprouse, J., and Almeida, D. (2013). “The role of experimental syntax in an integrated cognitive science of language,” in The Cambridge Hand book of Biolinguistics, eds C. Boeckx and K. K. Grohmann (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 181–202.

Google Scholar

Stavrakaki, S., and van der Lely, H. (2010). Production and comprehension of pronouns by Greek children with specific language impairment. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 28, 189–216. doi: 10.1348/026151010X485269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tager-Flusberg, H. (1981). On the nature of linguistic functioning in early infantile autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 11, 45–56. doi: 10.1007/BF01531340

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Terzi, A. (2005). “Comparative syntax and language disorders,” in The Oxford Hand Book of Comparative Syntax, eds G. Cinque and R. Kayne (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 110–137.

Google Scholar

Terzi, A., Marinis, T., and Francis, K. (2016). The interface of syntax with pragmatics and prosody in children with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 46, 2692–2706. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2811-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Terzi, A., Marinis, T., Francis, K., and Kotsopoulou, A. (2012). Crosslinguistic differences in autistic children's comprehension of pronouns: English vs. Greek. Proc. BUCLD 36, 607–619.

Google Scholar

Terzi, A., Marinis, T., Kotsopoulou, A., and Francis, K. (2014). Grammatical abilities of Greek-speaking children with autism. Lang. Acquist. 21, 4–44. doi: 10.1080/10489223.2013.855216

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Terzi, A., Zafeiri, A., Marinis, T., and Francis, K. (2017). Object clitics in the narratives of high-functioning children with autism. Proc. BUCLD 41, 637–650.

Google Scholar

Theodorou, E., and Grohmann, K. K. (2015). Object clitics in Cypriot Greek children with SLI. Lingua 161, 144–158. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2014.11.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomas, E., Demuth, K., Smith-Lock, K. M., and Petocz, P. (2015). Phonological and morphophonological effects on grammatical development in children with specific language impairment. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 50, 516–528. doi: 10.1111/1460-6984.12152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Toro, R., Konyukh, M., Delorme, R., Leblond, C., Chaste, P., Fauchereau, F., et al. (2010). Key role for gene dosage and synaptic homeostasis in autism spectrum disorders. Trends Genet. 26, 363–372. doi: 10.1016/j.tig.2010.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsakiridou, M. (2006). The linguistic profile of Down's syndrome subjects: evidence from wh-movement construction. SOAS Work. Pap. Linguist. 14, 227–248.

Google Scholar

Tsimpli, I. M., Kambanaros, M., and Grohmann, K. K. (2017a). “Language pathology,” in The Oxford Hand Book of Universal Grammar, ed I. G. Roberts (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 486–508.

Google Scholar

Tsimpli, I. M., Peristeri, E., and Andreou, M. (2017b). Object clitic production in monolingual and bilingual children with Specific Language Impairment: a comparison between elicited production and narratives. Linguist. Approac. Bilingual. 7, 394–430. doi: 10.1075/lab.15025.tsi

CrossRef Full Text

Tsimpli, I. M., and Stavrakaki, S. (1999). The effects of a morphosyntactic deficit in the determiner system: the case of a Greek SLI child. Lingua 108, 31–85. doi: 10.1016/S0024-3841(98)00041-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Heuvel, M. P., and Sporns, O. (2013). Network hubs in the human brain. Trends Cogn. Sci. 17, 683–696. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Lely, H. K. J. (2005). Domain-specific cognitive systems: insight from Grammatical-SLI. Trends Cogn. Sci. 9, 53–59. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verhoeven, J. S., Rommel, N., Prodi, E., Leemans, A., Zink, I., Vandewalle, E., et al. (2012). Is there a common neuroanatomical substrate of language deficit between autism spectrum disorder and specific language impairment? Cereb. Cortex 22, 2263–2271. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr292

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volden, J., Coolican, J., Garon, N., White, J., and Bryson, S. (2009). Pragmatic language in autism spectrum disorder: relationships to measures of ability and disability. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 39, 388–393. doi: 10.1007/s10803-008-0618-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wittke, K., Mastergeorge, A. M., Ozonoff, S., Rogers, S. J., and, Naigles, L. R. (2017). Grammatical language impairment in autism spectrum disorder: exploring language phenotypes beyond stand ardized testing. Front. Psychol. 8:532. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00532

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: distributed morphology, grammatical marker, linguistic phenotype, syntax, Autism spectrum disorders (ASD), Down Syndrome, specific language impairment (SLI)

Citation: Leivada E, Kambanaros M and Grohmann KK (2017) The Locus Preservation Hypothesis: Shared Linguistic Profiles across Developmental Disorders and the Resilient Part of the Human Language Faculty. Front. Psychol. 8:1765. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01765

Received: 15 July 2017; Accepted: 25 September 2017;
Published: 13 October 2017.

Edited by:

Ángel J. Gallego, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Spain

Reviewed by:

LouAnn Gerken, University of Arizona, United States
F. Sayako Earle, University of Delaware, United States

Copyright © 2017 Leivada, Kambanaros and Grohmann. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Evelina Leivada, evelina@biolinguistics.eu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.