Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 18 October 2017
Sec. Developmental Psychology

Chinese Preschool Children’s Socioemotional Development: The Effects of Maternal and Paternal Psychological Control

  • 1Department of Psychology, Capital Normal University, Beijing, China
  • 2Qingdao Mental Health Center, Qingdao, China
  • 3School of Public Health, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China
  • 4Institute of Developmental Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China

The present study examined the relative prediction and joint effects of maternal and paternal psychological control on children’s socioemotional development. A total of 325 preschool children between the ages of 34 and 57 months (M = 4 years 2 months) and their parents participated in the study. Fathers and mothers, respectively, reported their levels of psychological control and mothers evaluated the socioemotional development of children using two indicators (i.e., behavioral problems and prosocial behaviors). The results indicated that the relative predictive effects of maternal and paternal psychological control on children’s socioemotional development differed. Specifically, maternal psychological control was a significant predictor of children’s behavioral problems and prosocial behaviors, whereas the levels of paternal psychological control were unrelated to children’s socioemotional development. With regard to the combined effects of maternal and paternal psychological control, the results of ANOVAs and simple slope analysis both indicated that children would be at risk of behavioral problems as long as they had one highly psychologically controlling parent. High levels of paternal psychological control were associated with increased behavioral problems of children only when maternal psychological control was low. However, the association between maternal psychological control and children’s behavioral behaviors was significant, despite paternal psychological control.

Introduction

Parental psychological control refers to a set of intrusive parenting behaviors characterized by manipulation of children’s inner world such as guilt induction, love withdrawal, shaming, constraining children’s expressions, and stifling autonomy (Schaefer, 1965; Barber, 1996; Gao et al., 2016). Children with psychologically controlling parents feel forced to act, think, or feel in ways dictated by their parents (Soenens and Vansteenkiste, 2010). The developmental niche model holds that parenting behaviors are the pivotal subsystem of children’s physical and mental development (Super and Harkness, 1986). Psychological control serves as an important indicator of parenting quality; high levels of parental psychological control are associated with children’s social and psychological dysfunction (Barber and Harmon, 2002). For example, studies have demonstrated that children with highly psychologically controlling parents report less prosocial behaviors (Clark et al., 2015), more relational aggressive behaviors (Kuppens et al., 2013), as well as higher levels of anxiety (Settipani et al., 2013), and depression (El-Sheikh et al., 2010). Therefore, high parental psychological control represents an important risk factor in the socioemotional development of children.

The Relative Prediction of Maternal and Paternal Psychological Control

A number of existing research about parental psychological control has focused on mothers (e.g., Mills and Rubin, 1998; El-Sheikh et al., 2010) or combined maternal and paternal psychological control into an aggregated construct of parenting behaviors (e.g., Wang et al., 2007; Lai et al., 2014). The few studies that have examined the separate effects of maternal and paternal psychological control on children’s socioemotional development have yielded inconsistent results. For example, Hart et al. (2000) found that among Russian preschool children only maternal – and not paternal – psychological control had a significant association with children’s aggressive behaviors. Laible and Carlo (2004) indicated that low levels of rigid control from mothers were related to children’s sympathy, social competence, and self-worth, whereas paternal control was not a predictor of children’s adjustment. Those results were consistent with the understanding that maternal parenting behaviors play the dominant and primary role in children’s development. However, a recent longitudinal study has determined that both maternal and paternal psychological controls significantly predict changes in children’s behavioral problems (Lansford et al., 2014). The relevant findings support quite different conclusions, suggesting a need for further examination of this matter.

Another question that requires further exploration is whether the adverse effects of parental psychological control may be generalized to different cultures. Some studies have suggested that parental psychological control may be more common in Eastern countries, such as China, than in Western societies (Soenens and Vansteenkiste, 2010). In addition, psychological control could have distinct effects in different cultures. For example, shaming, which generally seems negative in West, is used as a moral socialization mechanism to teach children the social norms and requirements in Chinese culture (Helwig et al., 2014). The specific effects of parental psychological control in Chinese culture merit attention.

Finally, the existing literature primarily has focused on parental psychological control and adolescent development. Psychological control undermines children’s autonomous function, thereby increasing the risk of social and psychological dysfunction (Barber and Harmon, 2002; Soenens et al., 2012). Adolescence is characterized by rapid separation and independence. It seems reasonable to assume the psychological control, which would hinder children’s independent development, exert the most distinct effects on adolescents. However, self-determination theory holds the psychological need for autonomy, competence, and relatedness present throughout life (Ryan et al., 2006). The negative impacts of parental psychological control would appear at various ages. Consequently, it would appear crucial to extend the body of existing research to include earlier developmental periods (Soenens and Vansteenkiste, 2010).

The Joint Effects of Maternal and Paternal Psychological Control

Although the studies mentioned above included both parents’ psychological control, not all of them examined the joint effect or configuration (high or low levels of psychological control of both parents, or mixed levels of paternal and maternal psychological control) on children’s development. Therefore, the question remains of how children’s development may be impacted by discordant controlling parenting from their mothers and fathers.

With regard to the joint influences of parental psychological control, two distinct problems may be identified. The first is whether the existence of two highly psychologically controlling parents confers “dual risk” for children? The second is how the discordant parental psychological control (one parent employed high psychological control and another conducted low level of psychological control) influences the children development.

Although no research has been conducted to examine the combined effects of discordant parental psychological control on children development, several studies have examined concordant/discordant parenting patterns in relation to various constructs. For example, Simons and Conger (2007) claimed that children yielded the best outcomes in mental health and academic domains if both parents engaged in authoritative parenting and the worst outcomes if both parents engaged in non-authoritative parenting. Children with discordant parenting behaviors scored in the middle. Similarly, Kochanska and Kim (2013) examined the relationship between children’s attachment with their parents and future behavioral problems. The results showed that the children who had insecure attachment with both parents had significantly more behavioral problems than those who had secure attachment with both parents and those with mixed attachments. Having secured attachment with at least one parent appeared to have a significant beneficial effect.

The Aims of This Study

In addition, the present study has two aims. First was to explore the relative predictive value of maternal and paternal psychological control on the socioemotional development of preschool children aged 2 to 5 in contemporary Chinese families, using the two indicators of behavioral problems and prosocial behaviors. Second was to investigate the joint effects of maternal and paternal psychological control on children’s socioemotional development.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Study subjects were recruited from four kindergartens in Beijing, the capital city of China. A total of 325 preschool children (169 girls and 156 boys) and their parents participated in the study. The selection criteria were as follows: (a) the parents were married and (b) the household was comprised of a nuclear family. One parent from each family provided written agreement to participate the research.

Measures

Demographic Characteristics

Child’s age and gender, parents’ education, monthly family income, and family subjective social status (SSS) were collected. A total of 313 of these parents provided their educational levels: 24.3% of the children’s mothers had a Master’s degree or higher = 3, 63.1% had a bachelor’s or vocational college degree = 2, 10% had secondary education or less = 1 (M = 2.13, SD = 0.58), compared with 26.8, 63.95, and 9.3%, respectively, for fathers (M = 2.18, SD = 0.58). The t-tests were conducted to compare the families with full data and the families with missing data in terms of paternal and maternal psychological control and children’s socioemotional development. There were no differences between the two forms of families (in terms of paternal psychological control, Mfull data = 2.20, Mmissing data = 2.40, ΔM = 0.19, 95% CI = [-0.17, 0.56], t = 1.03, ns; in terms of maternal psychological control, Mfull data = 2.12, Mmissing data = 2.40, ΔM = 0.27, 95% CI = [-0.07, 0.61], t = 1.54, ns; in terms of children’s behavioral problems, Mfull data = 0.52, Mmissing data = 0.55, ΔM = 0.03, 95% CI = [-0.10, 0.17], t = 0.47, ns; and in terms of children’s prosocial behaviors, Mfull data = 1.47, Mmissing data = 1.43, ΔM = -0.04, 95% CI = [-0.26, 0.19], t = -0.33, ns).

A total of 308 of these parents provided their family monthly income: 6.8% of the families had monthly income no more than 6000 Yuan, 11.0% had monthly income from 6000 to 10000 Yuan, 31.5% had income from 10000 to 20000 Yuan, and 50.5% had monthly income more than 20000 Yuan. The t-tests were conducted to compare the families with full data and the families with missing data in terms of paternal and maternal psychological control and children’s socioemotional development. There were no differences between the two forms of families (in terms of paternal psychological control, Mfull data = 2.22, Mmissing data = 2.04, ΔM = -0.17, 95% CI = [-0.45, 0.10], t = -1.11, ns; in terms of maternal psychological control, Mfull data = 2.14, Mmissing data = 1.97, ΔM = -0.18, 95% CI = [-0.47, 0.11], t = 1.12, ns; in terms of children’s behavioral problems, Mfull data = 0.52, Mmissing data = 0.51, ΔM = -0.01, 95% CI = [-0.13, 0.10], t = -0.25, ns; and in terms of children’s prosocial behaviors, Mfull data = 1.47, Mmissing data = 1.53, ΔM = 0.06, 95% CI = [-0.13, 0.25], t = 0.66, ns).

Subjective social status (SSS) was defined as the individual’s perception of his own position in the social hierarchy (Sakurai et al., 2010). Several studies suggested that SSS may represent a stronger predictor of psychological stress (Sakurai et al., 2010) and children’s self-rated health (Goodman et al., 2007). The MacArthur Scale was adopted to assess parents’ SSS. A drawing of a ladder with 10 rungs was shown to participants with the description as follows: “the ladder represents the social classes where people stand in our society. At the top of the ladder are the people who are the best off, those who have the most income, most education, and best jobs. At the bottom are the people who are the worst off, those who have the lowest income, least education, and worst jobs or no job.” The participants were asked to mark the rung that best represents where they believe they stand on the ladder (Adler et al., 2000). As few parents choose the top and bottom rank, the 1∼3 ranks combined into low SSS group (coded as 1), 4∼7 ranks into middle SSS group (coded as 2), and 8∼10 SSS into high SSS group (coded as 3). Among the 303 parents provided this information, 4% parents chose low SSS, 87.5% chose middle SSS, and 8.6% chose high SSS. The t-tests were conducted to compare the families with full data and the families with missing data in terms of paternal and maternal psychological control and children’s socioemotional development. There were no differences between the two forms of families (in terms of paternal psychological control, Mfull data = 2.21, Mmissing data = 2.20, ΔM = 0.00, 95% CI = [-0.27, 0.28], t = 0.03, ns; in terms of maternal psychological control, Mfull data = 2.14, Mmissing data = 2.08, ΔM = 0.06, 95% CI = [-0.20, 0.31], t = 0.43, ns; in terms of children’s behavioral problems, Mfull data = 0.52, Mmissing data = 0.50, ΔM = 0.02, 95% CI = [-0.09, 0.12], t = -0.04, ns; and in terms of children’s prosocial behaviors, Mfull data = 1.47, Mmissing data = 1.47, ΔM = 0.00, 95% CI = [-0.17, 0.17], t = 0.30, ns).

Psychological Control

Psychological control was assessed with the Chinese version of the 18-item measure developed by Wang et al. (2007): 10 items identify guilt induction (e.g., I tell my child that he/she should feel guilty when he/she does not meet my expectations), 5 identify the withholding of love (e.g., I act cold and unfriendly if my child does something I do not like), and 3 identify authority assertion (e.g., I tell my child that what I want him/her to do what best for them and not question authority). Both fathers and mothers indicated how true each item was for themselves (1 = not at all true; 5 = very true). The psychological control value is the mean score derived from the 18 items: higher scores indicate greater psychological control. In this study, Cronbach’s alphas for mothers and fathers were 0.88 and 0.87, respectively.

Socioemotional Development

The 25-item Chinese version of Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) was used to assess behavioral problems and prosocial behaviors as two indicators that reflected children’s socioemotional development levels. This scale was created by Goodman (1997), and Du et al. (2006) revised the original to create a valid and reliable Chinese version. The instrument may be used to assess behavioral and emotional problems of children and adolescents. The SDQ includes five subscales: emotional symptoms subscale, conduct problems subscale, hyperactivity subscale, peer relationship subscale, and prosocial behavior subscale. The scores for the first 4 subscales can be summed to generate a total difficulties score (Goodman, 1997; Silva et al., 2015) and we took the mean of the first 4 subscales as an indicator of behavioral problems. Mothers were asked to rate their children on a 3-point scale (0 = does not apply, 1 = partially applies, and 2 = certainly applies). In the current study, Cronbach’s alphas for behavioral problems and prosocial behaviors scales were 0.74 and 0.68, respectively.

Results

Preliminary Analysis

We examined the correlations among the measured constructs and represented them in Table 1. Maternal psychological control was positively related to behavioral problems and negatively related to prosocial behaviors. Paternal psychological control had a significant positive correlation with children’s behavioral problems, whereas correlation with prosocial behaviors was not significant.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Descriptive statistics and correlations for measures constructs.

To examine the associations between the measured structures and demographic characteristics, we conducted analyses of variance (ANOVAs) with maternal and paternal psychological control, children’s behavioral problems and prosocial behaviors as the respective dependent variables and child gender, maternal and paternal education, and SSS as the between-subject factors. In terms of prosocial behaviors, the main effect of child gender was significant, with girls exhibiting more prosocial behaviors than boys (Mboy = 1.41, SD = 0.38, Mgirl = 1.52, SD = 0.38, ΔM = -0.11, 95% CI = [-0.20, -0.03], F(1,323) = 7.26, p < 0.01). With regard to behavioral problems, the main effect of SSS was significant [F(2,300) = 4.28, p < 0.05]. The estimated marginal means and 95% CIs for behavioral problems were 0.63 [0.50, 0.77] for the children with low SSS, 0.53 [0.50, 0.55] for the children with middle SSS, and 0.41 [0.32, 0.50] for children with high SSS. The children with high SSS displayed less behavioral problems than children with middle SSS (difference of estimated marginal means was -0.11, SE = 0.05, 95% CI [-0.21, -0.02], p < 0.05) and less than those with high SSS (difference of estimated marginal means was -0.22, SE = 0.08, 95% CI [-0.38, -0.06], p < 0.05). Therefore, the children’s gender and SSS were controlled in subsequent analyses.

Concordance Analysis between Maternal and Paternal Psychological Control

To examine the difference between maternal and paternal psychological control, a paired-samples t-test was conducted. The results showed that paternal psychological control was significantly higher than maternal psychological control (Mm = 2.13, SD = 0.59, Mp = 2.21, SD = 0.63; ΔM = -0.07, 95% CI = [-0.15, 0], t(324) = 1.97, p = 0.05). K-means cluster was conducted to separate maternal and paternal psychological control into a high-score and low-score group and create four joint psychological control classifications: low scores of both maternal and paternal psychological controls (n = 124, 38.2%); high scores of both maternal and paternal psychological controls (n = 81, 24.9%); high scores of maternal psychological control and low scores of paternal psychological control (n = 50, 15.4%); and low scores maternal psychological control and high scores of paternal psychological control (n = 70, 21.5%), see Table 2. An examination of the concordance of maternal and paternal psychological control revealed considerable discordance (Cohen’s k = 0.25); 36.9% (120/325) of the children experienced discordant parenting psychological control context. Additionally, an examination of the correlation between maternal and paternal psychological control revealed a relatively positive relation, r = 0.38, p < 0.01.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Classification of maternal and paternal psychological control.

Socioemotional Development of Children in Different Groups

Descriptive statistics for children’s socioemotional development is displayed in Table 3. To test the connection between maternal and paternal psychological control and socioemotional development, one-way ANOVA was computed with the two indicators of socioemotional development as dependent variables and the four-way classification of psychological control as the between-subject factor.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Descriptive statistics for children’s socioemotional development.

The data collected showed some interesting findings. The main effects of the organization of parental psychological control were significant for children behavioral problems [F(3,321) = 9.90, η2 = 0.09, p < 0.001]. The estimated marginal means and 95% CIs for behavioral problems were 0.44 [0.40, 0.48] for the children with both less controlling parents, 0.58 [0.52, 0.64] for the children with highly controlling mothers and less controlling fathers, 0.53 [0.47, 0.58] for the children with less controlling mothers and highly controlling fathers, and 0.60 [0.55, 0.65] for children with both highly controlling parents. Least significant difference (LSD) post hoc tests were used to examine pairwise differences among the four groups. Children in low maternal and paternal psychological control group had less behavioral problems than children in high maternal and paternal psychological control group (difference of estimated marginal means was -0.16, SE = 0.03, 95% CI [-0.22, -0.07], p < 0.01), and less than those who in low paternal psychological control – high maternal psychological control group (difference of estimated marginal means was -0.14, SE = 0.04, 95% CI [-0.22, -0.07], p < 0.01), and less than those who in high paternal psychological control – low maternal psychological control group (difference of estimated marginal means was -0.09, SE = 0.03, 95% CI [-0.15, -0.02], p < 0.01). Notably, however, there were no differences among the other three groups. Overall, children would be at risk of behavioral problems if they had one parent who deployed a high level of psychological control.

The estimated marginal means and 95% CIs for prosocial behaviors were 1.51 [1.44, 1.57] for the children with both less controlling parents, 1.46 [1.35, 1.57] for the children with highly controlling mothers and less controlling fathers, 1.49 [1.40, 1.59] for the children with less controlling mothers and highly controlling fathers, and 1.39 [1.31, 1.48] for children with both highly controlling parents. Psychological control classifications did not significantly impact prosocial behaviors [F(3,321) = 1.62, η2 = 0.02, p > 0.05]. There were no differences among the four groups with regard to prosocial behaviors.

Regression Analysis for Psychological Control and Socioemotional Development

To examine the relative prediction and combined effects of maternal and paternal psychological control, hierarchical multiple regressions for each of the children’s socioemotional outcomes were conducted. The data were standardized before regression analysis and the simple size was 325. First, the child’s gender (0 = boy, 1 = girl) and SSS (the missing data were replaced by means) was entered, followed by maternal and paternal psychological control scores, and finally, the interaction terms of maternal and paternal psychological control in order to examine the combined effects of parental psychological control. The results were reported in Table 4. As expected, maternal psychological control could predict children’s behavioral problems positively (b = 0.06, 95% CI [0.04, 0.09], p < 0.05) and prosocial behaviors negatively (b = -0.04, 95% CI [-0.09, 0.00], p < 0.1), whereas paternal psychological control was not a significant predictor for the two indicators of children’s socioemotional development. Moreover, the interaction term significantly predicted children’s behavioral problems (b = -0.03, 95% CI [-0.05, -0.00], p < 0.05), but not their prosocial behaviors (b = 0.02, 95% CI [-0.03, 0.06], p > 0.1); these results were consistent with the ANOVAs.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Regression analysis for parental psychological control predicting children’s socioemotional outcomes.

Follow-up simple slopes analysis was explored (Aiken and West, 1991). The graphical presentation of the product of maternal psychological control and paternal psychological control interaction is shown in Figure 1 (paternal psychological control as the independent variable and maternal psychological control as the moderator) and Figure 2 (maternal psychological control as the independent variable and maternal psychological control as the moderator). In Figure 1, the slope of the line representing high levels of maternal psychological control (+1 SD above the mean) was not significantly different from 0 [t(322) = -0.56, p = 0.56]. However, the slope of the line representing low levels of maternal psychological (-1 SD below the mean) was significantly different from 0 [t(322) = 2.13, p < 0.05]. For the children with a less psychologically controlling mother, high paternal psychological control was associated with an increase in behavioral problems. Nevertheless, the association was not significant for the children whose mother employed high levels of psychological control. In Figure 2, the slopes of the lines representing high levels of paternal psychological control (+1 SD above the mean) and low levels of paternal psychological control (-1 SD below the mean) were both significantly different from 0 (bhigh = 0.04, SE = 0.02, t(322) = 2.09, p < 0.05, and blow = 0.09, SE = 0.02, t(322) = 4.96, p < 0.001). Despite the level of paternal psychological control, maternal psychological control would consistently and positively predict children’s behavioral problems.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. The joint effect of paternal and maternal psychological control (paternal psychological control as the independent variable and maternal psychological control as the moderator).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. The joint effect of paternal and maternal psychological control (maternal psychological control as the independent variable and maternal psychological control as the moderator).

Discussion

This study examined the predictive power and combined effects of maternal and paternal psychological control on the socioemotional development of preschool children 2–5 years of age in China.

Despite some researchers considered psychological control as a commonly used or acceptable parenting and thought it could promote children’s moral socialization (Helwig et al., 2014), our results found that psychologically controlling parenting was harmful to children’s socioemotional development in China. This result suggested that the detrimental effects of psychological control were culture-general. Therefore, parents even in China should reduce the use of parenting practices involving psychological control.

The most striking findings were the differential effects of paternal and maternal psychological control. Results revealed that higher maternal psychological control could predict more behavioral problems significantly and less prosocial behaviors marginally significantly. There were no parallel findings for paternal psychological control. These results could imply that the effects of maternal psychological control on children’s socioemotional competence were more clear and robust than the effects of paternal psychological control in China. These findings were not consistent with previous research in other countries. For example, a cross-cultural study, which examined the association between parental psychological control and depressive symptoms in Belgian and South Korean adolescents, showed that both maternal and paternal psychological controls were significant indicators for childhood depression (Soenens et al., 2012). Additionally, Arim and Shapka (2008) found that both maternal and paternal psychological controls significantly predicted externalized behavioral problems of British, Colombian, and Canadian children.

Several explanations for the lesser effect of paternal psychological control could be offered. First, the involvement of Chinese fathers in early childhood was still low. Most fathers were engaged in work and spend less time with family and children. A study conducted in China reported that 78.5% fathers spend less than 2 h with their children every day. In 67.8% of families, mothers or grandparents were the main caregivers for preschool children (Wei, 2013, Unpublished). Therefore, the paternal controlling parenting may exert relatively weaker effects on preschool children’s socioemotional development. Second, several studies suggested that maternal and paternal parenting might have differential influence on various aspects of a child’s development (Laible and Carlo, 2004; Boldt et al., 2014). The current study only examined behavioral problems and prosocial behaviors as outcomes, while the paternal controlling parenting may be a significant predictor for other areas of children’s development. Father’s involvement may be important to the development of individual achievement, self-definition, and independence (Soenens et al., 2010). Research has found that, compared with maternal parenting, paternal behaviors have a greater influence on a child’s self-esteem and self-efficacy (Bean et al., 2003). Therefore, paternal controlling parenting might play a greater role for children’s development in the areas beyond those examined in this study. Third, the relationship between paternal psychological control and children’s socioemotional development may be moderated by other variables. For example, Murray et al. (2014) suggested that the quality of the mother–child relationship moderated the association between paternal psychological control and children’s aggressive behaviors. More specially, paternal psychological control predicted children’s aggression when they experienced low-quality relationships with their mothers. Consequently, the main effect of paternal psychological control on children’s socioemotional development could be hidden in interactions between controlling parenting and other factors.

The results of this study suggested that there was no difference among the four groups with regard to prosocial behaviors. However, children whose parents employed less psychological control showed less behavioral problems than children in the other three groups. The findings indicated that children would be at risk for behavioral problems as long as one parent engaged in high levels of psychological control. Moreover, the interaction of paternal and maternal psychological control was significant for the prediction of children’s behavioral problems. The graphical representation of the interaction clearly suggested that a combination of less psychological control by both parents resulted in the fewest behavioral problems. Interestingly, the effect with maternal control as the primary agent and paternal control as the moderator suggested that maternal psychological control was consistently and positively associated with children’s behavioral behaviors no matter the character of paternal psychological control. In contradistinction, paternal control as the main agent and maternal control as the moderator showed that an increase of paternal psychological control was associated with an increase of behavioral problems when maternal psychological control was low. However, that was not the case when maternal psychological control was high. These results contribute to an understanding of the specific mechanisms at work with paternal and maternal psychological control. Maternal psychological control exerts a consistently negative effect on children’s development. Paternal psychological control matters only in conjunction with less maternal psychological control.

Although the current study provided a better understanding of the different effects of paternal and maternal psychological control, several limitations of this study should be noted. First, the cross-sectional nature of the current study prevents us to clear the direction of effects between parental controlling parenting and children’s developmental problems. Longitudinal research would be needed to test the causal relationships among these variables. Second, the sample was relatively homogenous. All of them were recruited from Beijing, the political, economic, and cultural center of China. Almost half of the families jointed the study were at high socioeconomic status. Parents showed relatively low level of psychological control and children were well-behaved. Therefore, the low level of variability of variables reduced the ecological validity of this study. Future studies could make a complete examination of parental psychological control and children development in heterogeneous population. Third, the data on parental psychological control and children’s socioemotional development were obtained from parental self-reports; the relationships identified could reflect common method variance. Therefore, future studies could utilize different measuring tools and informants to assure greater validity of the results. Fourth, the possible interplay between parenting and children’s characteristics could exist. The differential susceptibility hypothesis assumes that some children have high developmental plasticity and will be more affected by parenting experiences than others due to their own characteristics (e.g., temperamental, physiological, or genetics; Belsky and Pluess, 2013; Belsky et al., 2015). Numerous researchers have noted that children’s temperaments had a moderate effect on the association between parenting and development. For example, Bradley and Corwyn (2008) suggested that there would be a stronger relationship between maternal sensitivity and children’s behavioral problems among children with difficult temperaments. Zarra-Nezhad et al. (2014) found that children who displayed a relatively high level of social withdrawal were more vulnerable to the negative effects of high parental psychological control. Therefore, future research should consider the moderating role of children’s characteristics in the relationship between controlling parenting and children’s development. Finally, this study primarily focused on the parental psychological control, a family predictor of children development. However, teachers are alternative caregivers and teacher–child relationship is another important factor in preschool children’s socioemotional development (Burchinal et al., 2002; Murray et al., 2008). Future study would take the family and school factors into consideration and make a more comprehensive exploration about children development.

Despite these limitations, several strengths of the study merit mention. First, the research benefited from data collected on psychological control by both mothers and fathers from the same families. This provided the opportunity to examine the relative effect and joint effects of paternal and maternal psychological control on children’s socioemotional development. Second, the findings expanded upon existing knowledge regarding paternal psychological control and children’s development, and contributed a greater understanding of the practical implications. Parental psychological control had adverse effects on preschool children’s socioemotional development in Chinese culture. The children were at the high risk of behavioral problems as long as one parent exerts high level of psychological control. Therefore, we recommend that both parents, especially mothers, should reduce their controlling parenting and employ more adaptive parenting to promote children’s socioemotional development.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Research Ethics Committee of Capital Normal University with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Research Ethics Committee of Capital Normal University.

Author Contributions

SX and XL conceived and designed the study. XG, XS, DZ, MZ, and BD performed the collection of data. XG analyzed and interpreted the data. SX, XL, and XG completed and modified the manuscript. MA provided language editing for the manuscript.

Funding

This study was supported by National Social Science Fund of China (grant no. 17BSH144) and MOE (Ministry of Education in China) Project of Humanities and Social Sciences (grant no. 16YJC190023).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Adler, N. E., Epel, E. S., Castellazzo, G., and Ickovics, J. R. (2000). Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: preliminary data in healthy white women. Health Psychol. 19, 586–592. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.19.6.586

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aiken, L. S., and West, S. G. (1991). Multiple Regression: Testing and Interpreting Interactions. Newbury, CA: Sage.

Google Scholar

Arim, R. G., and Shapka, J. D. (2008). The impact of pubertal timing and parental control on adolescent problem behaviors. J. Youth Adolesc. 37, 445–455. doi: 10.1007/s10964-006-9158-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barber, B. K. (1996). Parental psychological control: revisiting a neglected construct. Child Dev. 67, 3296–3319. doi: 10.2307/1131780

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barber, B. K., and Harmon, E. L. (2002). “Violating the self: parental psychological control of children and adolescents,” in Intrusive Parenting: How Psychological Control Affects Children and Adolescents, ed. B. K. Barber (Washington, DC: American Psychological Association), 15–52. doi: 10.1037/10422-002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bean, R. A., Bush, K. R., McKenry, P. C., and Wilson, S. M. (2003). The impact of parental support, behavioral control, and psychological control on the academic achievement and self-esteem of African American and European American adolescents. J. Adolesc. Res. 18, 523–541. doi: 10.1177/0743558403255070

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belsky, J., Newman, D. A., Widaman, K. F., Rodkin, P., Pluess, M., Fraley, R. C., et al. (2015). Differential susceptibility to effects of maternal sensitivity? A study of candidate plasticity genes. Dev. Psychopathol. 27, 725–746. doi: 10.1017/S0954579414000844

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belsky, J., and Pluess, M. (2013). Beyond risk, resilience, and dysregulation: phenotypic plasticity and human development. Dev. Psychopathol. 25, 1243–1261. doi: 10.1017/S095457941300059X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boldt, L. J., Kochanska, G., Yoon, J. E., and Nordling, J. K. (2014). Children’s attachment to both parents from toddler age to middle childhood: links to adaptive and maladaptive outcomes. Attach. Hum. Dev. 16, 211–229. doi: 10.1080/14616734.2014.889181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bradley, R. H., and Corwyn, R. F. (2008). Infant temperament, parenting, and externalizing behavior in first grade: a test of the differential susceptibility hypothesis. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 49, 124–131. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01829.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burchinal, M., Peisnerfeinberg, E., Pianta, R. C., and Howes, C. (2002). Development of academic skills from preschool through second grade: family and classroom predictors of developmental trajectories. J. School Psychol. 40, 415–436. doi: 10.1016/S0022-4405(02)00107-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, C., Dahlen, E. R., and Nicholson, B. C. (2015). The role of parenting in relational aggression and prosocial behavior among emerging adults. Attach. Hum. Dev. 24, 185–202. doi: 10.1080/10926771.2015.1002653

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Du, B. S., Kou, J. H., Wang, X. L., Xia, L. M., and Zou, R. H. (2006). A study on the strengths and difficulties questionnaire (SDQ). Psychol. Sci. 29, 1419–1421.

Google Scholar

El-Sheikh, M., Hinnant, J. B., Kelly, R. J., and Erath, S. (2010). Maternal psychological control and child internalizing symptoms: vulnerability and protective factors across bioregulatory and ecological domains. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 51, 188–198. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02140.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, X., Xing, S. F., and Zhao, J. Y. (2016). Parental psychological control and the relationship with children’s psychosocial functioning. Adv. Psychol. Sci. 24, 1792–1801. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2016.01792

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodman, E., Huang, B., Schaferkalkhoff, T., and Adler, N. E. (2007). Perceived socioeconomic status: a new type of identity that influences adolescents’ self-rated health. J. Adolesc. Health 41, 479–487. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.05.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodman, R. (1997). The strengths and difficulties questionnaire: a research note. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 38, 581–586. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01545.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hart, C. H., Nelson, D. A., Robinson, C. C., Olsen, S. F., McNeilly-Choque, M. K., Porter, C. L., et al. (2000). “Russian parenting styles and family processes: linkages with subtypes of victimization and aggression,” in Family and Peers: Linking Two Social Worlds, eds K. A. Kerns, J. M. Contreras, A. M. Neal-Barnett, K. A. Kerns, J. M. Contreras, and A. M. Neal-Barnett (Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers), 47–84.

Google Scholar

Helwig, C. C., To, S., Wang, Q., Liu, C., and Yang, S. (2014). Judgments and reasoning about parental discipline involving induction and psychological control in China and Canada. Child Dev. 85, 1150–1167. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kochanska, G., and Kim, S. (2013). Early attachment organization with both parents and future behavior problems: from infancy to middle childhood. Child Dev. 84, 283–296. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01852.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuppens, S., Laurent, L., Heyvaert, M., and Onghena, P. (2013). Associations between parental psychological control and relational aggression in children and adolescents: a multilevel and sequential meta-analysis. Dev. Psychol. 49, 1697–1712. doi: 10.1037/a0030740

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lai, X. F., Zhang, W., Bao, Z. Y., Wang, Y. H., and Xiong, Q. L. (2014). Parental psychological control and depression among adolescents: a moderated mediation model. Psychol. Dev. Educ. 30, 293–302.

Laible, D. J., and Carlo, G. (2004). The differential relations of maternal and paternal support and control to adolescent social competence, self-worth, and sympathy. J. Adolesc. Res. 19, 759–782. doi: 10.1177/0743558403260094

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lansford, J. E., Laird, R. D., Pettit, G. S., Bates, J. E., and Dodge, K. A. (2014). Mothers’ and fathers’ autonomy-relevant parenting: longitudinal links with adolescents’ externalizing and internalizing behavior. J. Youth Adolesc. 43, 1877–1889. doi: 10.1007/s10964-013-0079-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mills, R. L., and Rubin, K. H. (1998). Are behavioral and psychological control both differentially associated with childhood aggression and social withdrawal? Can. J. Behav. Sci. 30, 132–136. doi: 10.1037/h0085803

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, C., Murray, K. M., and Waas, G. A. (2008). Child and teacher reports of teacher-student relationships: concordance of perspectives and associations with school adjustment in Urban kindergarten classrooms. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 29, 49–61. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2007.10.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, K. W., Dwyer, K. M., Rubin, K. H., Knighton-Wisor, S., and Booth-LaForce, C. (2014). Parent–child relationships, parental psychological control, and aggression: maternal and paternal relationships. J. Youth Adolesc. 43, 1361–1373. doi: 10.1007/s10964-013-0019-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryan, R. M., Deci, E. L., Grolnick, W. S., and La Guardia, J. G. (2006). “The significance of autonomy and autonomy support in psychological development and psychopathology,” in Developmental psychopathology, Theory and Methods, 2nd Edn, Vol. 1, eds D. Cicchetti and D. Cohen (New York: Wiley), 795–849.

Google Scholar

Sakurai, K., Kawakami, N., Yamaoka, K., Ishikawa, H., and Hashimoto, H. (2010). The impact of subjective and objective social status on psychological distress among men and women in Japan. Soc. Sci. Med. 70, 1832–1839. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2010.01.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaefer, E. S. (1965). Children’s reports of parental behavior: an inventory. Child Dev. 36, 413–424. doi: 10.2307/1126465

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Settipani, C. A., O’Neil, K. A., Podell, J. L., Beidas, R. S., and Kendall, P. C. (2013). Youth anxiety and parent factors over time: directionality of change among youth treated for anxiety. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 42, 9–21. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2012.719459

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Silva, T. B., Osorio, F. D., and Loureiro, S. R. (2015). SDQ: discriminative validity and diagnostic potential. Front. Psychol. 6:811. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00811

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simons, L. G., and Conger, R. D. (2007). Linking mother-father differences in parenting to a typology of family parenting styles and adolescent outcomes. J. Fam. Issues 28, 212–241. doi: 10.1177/0192513X06294593

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soenens, B., Park, S., Vansteenkiste, M., and Mouratidis, A. (2012). Perceived parental psychological control and adolescent depressive experiences: a cross-cultural study with Belgian and South-Korean adolescents. J. Adolesc. 35, 261–272. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2011.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soenens, B., and Vansteenkiste, M. (2010). A theoretical upgrade of the concept of parental psychological control: proposing new insights on the basis of self-determination theory. Dev. Rev. 30, 74–99. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2009.11.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soenens, B., Vansteenkiste, M., and Luyten, P. (2010). Toward a domain-specific approach to the study of parental psychological control: distinguishing between dependency-oriented and achievement-oriented psychological control. J. Pers. 78, 217–256. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00614.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Super, C. M., and Harkness, S. (1986). The developmental niche: a conceptualization at the interface of child and culture. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 9, 545–569. doi: 10.1177/016502548600900409

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Q., Pomerantz, E. M., and Chen, H. (2007). The role of parents’ control in early adolescents’ psychological functioning: a longitudinal investigation in the United States and China. Child Dev. 78, 1592–1610. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01085.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zarra-Nezhad, M., Kiuru, N., Aunola, K., Zarra-Nezhad, M., Ahonen, T., Poikkeus, A. M., et al. (2014). Social withdrawal in children moderates the association between parenting styles and the children’s own socioemotional development. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 55, 1260–1269. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: psychological control, socioemotional development, behavioral problems, prosocial behaviors, preschool children

Citation: Xing S, Gao X, Song X, Archer M, Zhao D, Zhang M, Ding B and Liu X (2017) Chinese Preschool Children’s Socioemotional Development: The Effects of Maternal and Paternal Psychological Control. Front. Psychol. 8:1818. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01818

Received: 22 July 2017; Accepted: 30 September 2017;
Published: 18 October 2017.

Edited by:

Yusuke Moriguchi, Kyoto University, Japan

Reviewed by:

Kimberly Day, University of West Florida, United States
Claudio Longobardi, Università degli Studi di Torino, Italy

Copyright © 2017 Xing, Gao, Song, Archer, Zhao, Zhang, Ding and Liu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Xia Liu, liuxia@bnu.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.