Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Psychol., 14 November 2017
Sec. Performance Science

Eysenck's Theory of Personality and the Role of Background Music in Cognitive Task Performance: A Mini-Review of Conflicting Findings and a New Perspective

  • Institut für Musikwissenschaft und Medienwissenschaft, Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany

The question of whether background music is able to enhance cognitive task performance is of interest to scholars, educators, and stakeholders in business alike. Studies have shown that background music can have beneficial, detrimental or no effects on cognitive task performance. Extraversion—and its postulated underlying cause, cortical arousal—is regarded as an important factor influencing the outcome of such studies. According to Eysenck's theory of personality, extraverts' cortical arousal at rest is lower compared to that of introverts. Scholars have thus hypothesized that extraverts should benefit from background music in cognitive tasks, whereas introverts' performance should decline with music in the background. Reviewing studies that have considered extraversion as a mediator of the effect of background music on cognitive task performance, it is demonstrated that there is as much evidence in favor as there is against Eysenck's theory of personality. Further, revisiting Eysenck's concept of cortical arousal—which has traditionally been assessed by activity in the EEG alpha band—and reviewing literature on the link between extraversion and cortical arousal, it is revealed that there is conflicting evidence. Due to Eysenck's focus on alpha power, scholars have largely neglected higher frequency bands in the EEG signal as indicators of cortical arousal. Based on recent findings, it is suggested that beta power might not only be an indicator of alertness and attention but also a predictor of cognitive task performance. In conclusion, it is proposed that focused music listening prior to cognitive tasks might be a more efficient way to boost performance than listening to background music during cognitive tasks.

The Effects of Background Music on Cognitive Task Performance

There has been a surge in commercial applications promising to improve their users' concentration and focus by playing specifically designed music in the background. The basic idea is simple: playing background music activates your brain and leads to better performance in cognitive tasks. However, this idea comes with several issues. For example, even if music is specifically designed to set free cognitive resources it is implausible that every person benefits in the same manner in a cognitive task. A piece of music that has beneficial effects on cognitive task performance for one individual may well have no effect, or even detrimental effects, for another. Is searching for background music that enhances cognitive task performance therefore chasing a red herring? Not necessarily, if—as a first step—we are able to identify and better understand the neural underpinnings that enhance cognitive task performance in general. As a second step, we may begin to ask what are the characteristics of the music needed to change an individual's neural activation in a specific way. Since inter-individual differences play an important role in this endeavor, the evidence in favor of, and against, Eysenck's theory of personality will be reviewed before a new perspective is provided.

Although the effects of background music on cognitive task performance have been studied by psychologists and educators for more than seventy years (Fendrick, 1937), no clear pattern of results has emerged thus far. On the one hand, background music, in comparison with silence, has been found to be beneficial for reading comprehension (Kiger, 1989), foreign vocabulary learning (de Groot, 2006; Kang and Williamson, 2014), spatial and linguistic processing (Angel et al., 2010), IQ tests (Cockerton et al., 1997), spatial and numerical reasoning (Miller and Schyb, 1989), visual search tasks (Crust et al., 2004), and students' achievement in a psychology class (Schreiber, 1988). On the other hand, background music, compared to silence, has been found to impair cognitive performance, showing detrimental effects for reading comprehension (Fendrick, 1937; Henderson et al., 1945; Etaugh and Ptasnik, 1982; Furnham and Bradley, 1997; Avila et al., 2012; Thompson et al., 2012), verbal memory (Iwanaga and Ito, 2002; Woo and Kanachi, 2005; Cassidy and MacDonald, 2007), visual memory (Furnham and Bradley, 1997), serial recall of digits (Nittono, 1997; Alley and Greene, 2008), Stroop tasks (Parente, 1976; Cassidy and MacDonald, 2007), writing fluency (Ransdell and Gilroy, 2001), and logical reasoning and associative learning (Crawford and Strapp, 1994). Yet other investigations revealed that background music had no significant impact on cognitive task performance whatsoever (Henderson et al., 1945; Freeburne and Fleischer, 1952; Furnham and Allass, 1999; Pool et al., 2003; Alley and Greene, 2008; Schlittmeier and Hellbrück, 2009; Thompson et al., 2012). A recent meta-analysis on the effects of background music on adults' cognitive, affective and behavioral responses seems to support the trend toward an overall null effect (Kämpfe et al., 2011).

Without theory-driven research focused on inter-individual differences, these contradictory findings are not surprising. To address this issue, many scholars have used Eysenck's theory of personality (Eysenck, 1967) as a theoretical framework for their studies. While there are several inter-individual differences that influence the effects of background music on cognitive task performance—ranging from personality traits to musical taste and age—one inter-individual difference that has been studied extensively is extraversion.

According to a particular aspect of Eysenck's theory of personality, extraversion can be described and explained by the underlying cortical arousal. Extraverts are reported to have a lower level of cortical arousal compared to introverts. Eysenck's theory therefore predicts that introverts require little or no external stimulation to reach an optimal level of cognitive performance, whereas extraverts require comparatively more external stimulation. External stimulation exceeding the optimal threshold should lead to a decline in cognitive performance, following the Yerkes-Dodson Law (Yerkes and Dodson, 1908). Thus, presented with moderate to high levels of external stimulation should lead to a decline in introverts', but not extraverts', cognitive performance.

Using background music as a source of external stimulation—which has been shown to increase arousal in participants in several studies (Thompson et al., 2001; Jones et al., 2006; Schellenberg et al., 2007)—researchers have empirically tested Eysenck's theory by investigating intro- and extraverts' performance in different cognitive tasks.

Introverts' and Extraverts' Performance in Cognitive Tasks with Background Music and Silence

There is a substantial amount of evidence in favor of Eysenck's theory of personality, as the following studies show. Reporting a clear cross-over interaction between extraversion and background condition (either silence, simple music or complex music), Furnham and Allass (1999) showed that introverts' performance in two memory tests—immediate and delayed recall of visual objects—was best during silence and poorest with complex music, whereas extraverts performed best with complex music and poorest during silence. However, most evidence supporting Eysenck's theory of personality reveals music's detrimental effect on introverts' performance rather than music's beneficial effect on extraverts' performance in comparison with silence. For instance, testing introverts and extraverts during silence or with pop music in the background, Furnham and Bradley (1997) found that introverts who carried out a memory test in silence performed better than introverts presented with pop music. The same authors also showed that introverts completing a reading comprehension task performed poorer in the presence of music compared to silence, whereas extraverts showed no difference. Other studies have provided similar findings for reading comprehension tasks: Daoussis and McKelvie (1986) reported that introverts showed a poorer performance with rock'n'roll music in the background compared to silence, whereas extraverts did not differ in these two conditions. Furnham and Strbac (2002) showed that introverts performed poorer with music or office noise in the background compared to silence, whereas no difference between these three conditions was found in extraverts. Other cognitive tasks have produced comparable results. Introverts showed a linear decline in performance from silence to simple, and then complex, music in a spatial reasoning task (Furnham and Allass, 1999), and Cassidy and MacDonald (2007) revealed that, compared to silence, the presence of highly arousing music with negative affect—as well as the presence of background noise—led to a poorer performance of introverts compared to extraverts in a Stroop task. Another study (Dobbs et al., 2011) showed that extraversion was a significant predictor of performance in an abstract reasoning task and in a test of general cognitive ability when music or noise was present in the background. The more introvert a participant was the poorer their performance under these conditions—especially during noise—whereas task completion in silence revealed no, or only very weak, differential effects. Further indirect evidence was provided by Crawford and Strapp (1994) who tested a sample of students reporting usually studying either with or without background music. Those studying without background music showed a linear decline in performance in an associative memory task from silence to instrumental, and then vocal, music, whereas those studying with background music showed no clear pattern. In accordance with Eysenck's theory of personality, the latter group scored significantly higher on an extraversion scale than those studying without background music.

Even though these studies form a substantial body of evidence in favor of Eysenck's theory of personality, there are also several studies that have failed to support his theory. Testing intro- and extraverts, neither Furnham et al. (1999) nor Avila et al. (2012) found a significant interaction between extraversion and background condition—either vocal music, instrumental music or silence—in any of the following tests: reading comprehension tasks, logical reasoning, a coding task, a numerical test or a diagrammatic test (see also Furnham and Allass, 1999; Kou et al., 2017). Nor did Chamorro-Premuzic et al. (2009) find such interaction effect in logical reasoning or verbal tasks. Absence of the interaction between extraversion and background condition was further documented in arithmetic tasks and prose recall (Furnham and Strbac, 2002), and the hypothesized interaction was also absent in various memory tasks: immediate, delayed and free recall of verbal items (Cassidy and MacDonald, 2007), as well as immediate recall of visual objects (Furnham and Bradley, 1997).

Although taking into account inter-individual differences is vital when studying the effects of background music on cognitive task performance, the conflicting findings seem to suggest that extraversion, as measured with standard questionnaires alone, does not lead to conclusive results (see overview in Table 1). In an attempt to start disentangling these mixed findings, researchers have considered a more objective way of assessing inter-individual differences, i.e., investigating what Eysenck regarded as the underlying cause of differences in extraversion: cortical arousal (for a review see Matthews and Gilliland, 1999).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Studies testing intro- and extraverts' performance in cognitive tasks under various background conditions.

Extraversion and Cortical Arousal in the EEG Alpha and Beta Bands

Eysenck himself (Hagemann et al., 1999) suggested that differences in extraversion are reflected in the basal level of cortical arousal, hypothesizing that extraverts possess a lower basal level compared to introverts. Traditionally, cortical arousal is measured as alpha power in the electroencephalogram. Researchers have long held the view that low alpha power (8–13 Hz) is associated with high mental activity (Ray and Cole, 1985; Schmidtke and Heller, 2004). In other words, more alpha power is an indicator of an idle neural state. Although Ray and Cole (1985) have argued that this arousal model simplifies the actual mechanisms by providing evidence that alpha power is related to attentional processes, whereas beta power (14–35 Hz)—normally associated with wakefulness and alertness—reflects emotional or cognitive processes, alpha power is still often used as a measure of cortical arousal, possibly because Eysenck himself (1994, p. 167, as cited in Matthews and Gilliland, 1999) regarded EEG, and particularly alpha power, as the “standard measure of cortical arousal.”

There is empirical support for Eysenck's claims. For instance, in a study measuring the same participants' basal cortical arousal three times over the course of several weeks to ensure variance introduced by external factors such as time of day or emotional events is minimized, Hagemann and colleagues (Hagemann et al., 2009) revealed that extraverts show more alpha power (i.e., less cortical arousal) than introverts. Similarly, Gale et al. (1969) reported more activity in the lower alpha range (7.5–10.5 Hz) in extraverts compared to introverts during a baseline measure of cortical arousal with eyes closed, as well as more alpha power using a gross filter (8–13 Hz) during moderate levels of external visual stimulation. Asking participants to empathize with positive and negative facial expressions while recording EEG data, Gale et al. (2001) showed again more alpha power in the lower (8–10 Hz) but not the higher alpha (10–12 Hz) band in extraverts compared to introverts. More support for Eysenck's theory of personality comes from Smith et al. (1995) who reported that introverts show less activity in the alpha band (i.e., more cortical arousal) than extraverts during presentation of positive, negative or neutral non-verbal auditory stimuli.

On the other hand, only weak evidence in favor of Eysenck's theory was provided by Beauducel et al. (2006) who found no significant effect in extraverts during a 40-min monotonous vigilance task. Furthermore, using baseline measures of cortical arousal, neither Hagemann et al. (1999) nor Schmidtke and Heller (2004) were able to find a relationship between alpha power and extraversion. Furthermore, Matthews and Amelang (1993) found no association between alpha power and extraversion, neither in any of three experimental conditions separately—silence with eyes closed, visual fixation and mental arithmetic with eyes closed—nor averaged across them.

Taken together, these findings suggest that alpha power might not be the most appropriate frequency range as an indicator of cortical arousal, especially during baseline levels of arousal. A possibly better alternative but less understood measure of cortical arousal is beta power as suggested by Ray and Cole (1985). Several studies have used beta power as an indicator of cortical arousal or alertness (Gale et al., 1969; Matthews and Amelang, 1993; Cardenas et al., 1997; Rangaswamy et al., 2002; Gram et al., 2005). Regarding cortical arousal and extraversion three studies are relevant here. Gale et al. (1969) reported more beta power in extraverts than introverts during a baseline measure of cortical activity with eyes closed. Similarly, Matthews and Amelang (1993) found a positive correlation between extraversion and beta power during a condition of moderate external stimulation. Importantly, these two findings are opposite to what one might expect based on Eysenck's theory of personality. Since high beta is supposed to be related to high cortical arousal, one would expect extraverts to show less beta power than introverts. Matthews and Amelang suggest that this assumption—high beta associated with high cortical arousal—could be wrong and speculate that high cortical arousal might as well be indicated by low beta power. However, there is at least one study that shows the predicted relationship. Gram et al. (2005) tested intro- and extraverts during a 2-min baseline condition with eyes open. In line with Eysenck's theory of personality the authors showed that extraverts exhibit more alpha power and less beta power than introverts. The distinction between the two groups was clearly present in higher beta bandwidths (26–39 Hz), but less clear in low beta (13–25 Hz), suggesting that finer beta filters might be needed to find the hypothesized relationship between beta activity and extraversion.

To sum up, it should have become obvious that no clear link between extraversion and cortical arousal currently exists. Although alpha power has traditionally been used as an indicator of cortical arousal to distinguish between intro- and extraverts, this might not be the most suitable neural correlate of cortical arousal. Given the contradictory literature on the effects of background music on cognitive task performance, the relationship between extraversion, alpha power and task performance is obscure. What might elucidate this situation is beta power as an indicator of cortical arousal, even though the role of beta power for cognitive task performance, let alone its relationship with background music, is still poorly understood.

A New Perspective: Music, Beta Power and Cognitive Task Performance

There is evidence that beta power increases during cognitive tasks, e.g., in a sustained attention test (Molteni et al., 2007) or during reading and subtraction tasks (Fitzgibbon et al., 2004). What is more, a recent study provided evidence that more beta power is associated with enhanced cognitive performance. Küssner et al. (2016) showed that beta power predicts the number of correctly recalled words in a foreign-vocabulary learning task. EEG was measured in silence before the learning task, thus hinting at the importance of the level of cortical arousal prior to a learning task. The authors also used background music to induce cortical arousal in extra- and introverts but found no evidence in favor of Eysenck's theory of personality.

The effect of beta power on cognitive task performance opens up new questions. First, we need a better understanding of the role of beta power in cognitive task performance. Due to Eysenck's focus on alpha power, many scholars have used alpha rather than beta power to assess cortical arousal, neglecting higher frequency bands in the EEG signal. To investigate empirically whether beta power predicts performance one could measure beta power as a predictor in a regression model with cognitive task performance as outcome variable. Secondly, if beta power turns out to be a significant predictor of performance—perhaps even in different types of cognitive tasks—a logical follow-up question is: how can we enhance beta power prior to these tasks? Rather than listening to music in the background during a cognitive task, which may or may not enhance performance, it might be worthwhile investigating whether a short period of focused listening to music can stimulate our brain, possibly via enhanced beta power, such that our performance in a subsequent cognitive task is enhanced. There is even some evidence that (focused) listening to music can enhance beta power (Nakamura et al., 1999), which could be monitored in experimental setups with EEG biofeedback methods (Egner and Gruzelier, 2004). However, more research is needed to establish the conditions under which music listening affects beta power, including the type of music, current mood of the listener and possibly social factors as well. In a similar vein, stable and transient inter-individual differences, contextual features and the type of assignment will continue to be important factors for predicting the performance in cognitive tasks. But maybe focused music listening has the power to get us “in the zone” in a way of which background music is incapable.

Author Contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Alley, T. R., and Greene, M. E. (2008). The relative and perceived impact of irrelevant speech, vocal music and non-vocal music on working memory. Curr. Psychol. 27, 277–289. doi: 10.1007/s12144-008-9040-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Angel, L. A., Polzella, D. J., and Elvers, G. C. (2010). Background music and cognitive performance. Percept. Mot. Skills 110, 1059–1064. doi: 10.2466/pms.110.3c.1059-1064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Avila, C., Furnham, A., and McClelland, A. (2012). The influence of distracting familiar vocal music on cognitive performance of introverts and extraverts. Psychol. Music 40, 84–93. doi: 10.1177/0305735611422672

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beauducel, A., Brocke, B., and Leue, A. (2006). Energetical bases of extraversion: effort, arousal, EEG, and performance. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 62, 212–223. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cardenas, V. A., Gill, P., and Fein, G. (1997). Human P50 suppression is not affected by variations in wakeful alertness. Biol. Psychiatry 41, 891–901. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00186-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cassidy, G., and MacDonald, R. A. R. (2007). The effect of background music and background noise on the task performance of introverts and extraverts. Psychol. Music 35, 517–537. doi: 10.1177/0305735607076444

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chamorro-Premuzic, T., Swami, V., Terrado, A., and Furnham, A. (2009). The effects of background auditory interference and extraversion on creative and cognitive task performance. Int. J. Psychol. Stud. 1, 2–9. doi: 10.5539/ijps.v1n2p2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cockerton, T., Moore, S., and Norman, D. (1997). Cognitive test performance and background music. Percept. Mot. Skills 85, 1435–1438. doi: 10.2466/pms.1997.85.3f.1435

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crawford, H. J., and Strapp, C. M. (1994). Effects of vocal and instrumental music on visuospatial and verbal performance as moderated by studying preference and personality. Pers. Individ. Dif. 16, 237–245. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(94)90162-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crust, L., Clough, P. J., and Robertson, C. (2004). Influence of music and distraction on visual search performance of participants with high and low affect intensity. Percept. Mot. Skills 98, 888–896. doi: 10.2466/pms.98.3.888-896

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daoussis, L., and McKelvie, S. J. (1986). Musical preferences and effects of music on a reading comprehension test for extraverts and introverts. Percept. Mot. Skills 62, 283–289. doi: 10.2466/pms.1986.62.1.283

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Groot, A. M. B. (2006). Effects of stimulus characteristics and background music on foreign language vocabulary learning and forgetting. Lang. Learn. 56, 463–506. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9922.2006.00374.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dobbs, S., Furnham, A., and McClelland, A. (2011). The effect of background music and noise on the cognitive test performance of introverts and extraverts. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 25, 307–313. doi: 10.1002/acp.1692

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Egner, T., and Gruzelier, J. H. (2004). EEG Biofeedback of low beta band components: frequency-specific effects on variables of attention and event-related brain potentials. Clin. Neurophysiol. 115, 131–139. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(03)00353-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Etaugh, C., and Ptasnik, P. (1982). Effects of studying to music and post-study relaxation on reading comprehension. Percept. Mot. Skills 55, 141–142. doi: 10.2466/pms.1982.55.1.141

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eysenck, H. J. (1967). The Biological Basis of Personality. Springfield, IL: Thomas.

Google Scholar

Fendrick, P. (1937). The influence of music distraction upon reading efficiency. J. Educ. Res. 31, 264–271. doi: 10.1080/00220671.1937.10880749

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fitzgibbon, S. P., Pope, K. J., Mackenzie, L., Clark, C. R., and Willoughby, J. O. (2004). Cognitive tasks augment gamma EEG power. Clin. Neurophysiol. 115, 1802–1809. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2004.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Freeburne, C. M., and Fleischer, M. S. (1952). The effect of music distraction upon reading rate and comprehension. J. Educ. Psychol. 43, 101–109. doi: 10.1037/h0054219

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furnham, A., and Allass, K. (1999). The influence of musical distraction of varying complexity on the cognitive performance of extroverts and introverts. Eur. J. Pers. 13, 27–38. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-0984(199901/02)13:1<27::aid-per318>3.0.co;2-R

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furnham, A., and Bradley, A. (1997). Music while you work: the differential distraction of background music on the cognitive test performance of introverts and extraverts. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 11, 445–455. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-0720(199710)11:5<445::aid-acp472>3.0.co;2-r

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furnham, A., and Strbac, L. (2002). Music is as distracting as noise: the differential distraction of background music and noise on the cognitive test performance of introverts and extraverts. Ergonomics 45, 203–217. doi: 10.1080/00140130210121932

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Furnham, A., Trew, S., and Sneade, I. (1999). The distracting effects of vocal and instrumental music on the cognitive test performance of introverts and extraverts. Pers. Individ. Dif. 27, 381–392. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00249-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gale, A., Coles, M., and Blaydon, J. (1969). Extraversion–introversion and the EEG. Br. J. Psychol. 60, 209–223. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1969.tb01194.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gale, A., Edwards, J., Morris, P., Moore, R., and Forrester, D. (2001). Extraversion–introversion, neuroticism–stability, and EEG indicators of positive and negative empathic mood. Pers. Individ. Dif. 30, 449–461. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00036-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gram, P. C., Dunn, B. R., and Ellis, D. (2005). Relationship between EEG and psychological type. J. Psychol. Type 65, 33–46. Available online at: http://psycnet.apa.org/record/2006-10548-001

Google Scholar

Hagemann, D., Hewig, J., Walter, C., Schankin, A., Danner, D., and Naumann, E. (2009). Positive evidence for Eysenck's arousal hypothesis: a combined EEG and MRI study with multiple measurement occasions. Pers. Individ. Dif. 47, 717–721. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.06.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hagemann, D., Naumann, E., Lürken, A., Becker, G., Maier, S., and Bartussek, D. (1999). EEG asymmetry, dispositional mood and personality. Pers. Individ. Dif. 27, 541–568. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(98)00263-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Henderson, M. T., Crews, A., and Barlow, J. (1945). A study of the effect of music distraction on reading efficiency. J. Appl. Psychol. 29, 313–317. doi: 10.1037/h0056128

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Iwanaga, M., and Ito, T. (2002). Disturbance effect of music on processing of verbal and spatial memories. Percept. Mot. Skills 94, 1251–1258. doi: 10.2466/pms.2002.94.3c.1251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, M. H., West, S. D., and Estell, D. B. (2006). The Mozart effect: arousal, preference, and spatial performance. Psychol. Aesthetics Creativity Arts S1, 26–32. doi: 10.1037/1931-3896.S.1.26

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kämpfe, J., Sedlmeier, P., and Renkewitz, F. (2011). The impact of background music on adult listeners: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Music 39, 424–448. doi: 10.1177/0305735610376261

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kang, H. J., and Williamson, V. J. (2014). Background music can aid second language learning. Psychol. Music 42, 728–747. doi: 10.1177/0305735613485152

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kiger, D. M. (1989). Effects of music information load on a reading comprehension task. Percept. Mot. Skills 69, 531–534. doi: 10.2466/pms.1989.69.2.531

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kou, S., McClelland, A., and Furnham, A. (2017). The effect of background music and noise on the cognitive test performance of Chinese introverts and extraverts. Psychol. Music. doi: 10.1177/0305735617704300. [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Küssner, M. B., de Groot, A. M. B., Hofman, W. F., and Hillen, M. A. (2016). EEG beta power but not background music predicts the recall scores in a foreign-vocabulary learning task. PLoS ONE 11:e0161387. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0161387

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matthews, G., and Amelang, M. (1993). Extraversion, arousal theory and performance: a study of individual differences in the eeg. Pers. Individ. Dif. 14, 347–363. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(93)90133-N

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Matthews, G., and Gilliland, K. (1999). The personality theories of H.J. Eysenck and J.A. Gray: a comparative review. Pers. Individ. Diff. 26, 583–626.

Google Scholar

Miller, L. K., and Schyb, M. (1989). Facilitation and interference by background music. J. Music Ther. 26, 42–54. doi: 10.1093/jmt/26.1.42

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molteni, E., Bianchi, A. M., Butti, M., Reni, G., and Zucca, C. (2007). “Analysis of the dynamical behaviour of the EEG rhythms during a test of sustained attention,” in Paper Presented at the 29th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (Lyon).

Google Scholar

Nakamura, S., Sadato, N., Oohashi, T., Nishina, E., Fuwamoto, Y., and Yonekura, Y. (1999). Analysis of music–brain interaction with simultaneous measurement of regional cerebral blood flow and electroencephalogram beta rhythm in human subjects. Neurosci. Lett. 275, 222–226. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3940(99)00766-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nittono, H. (1997). Background instrumental music and serial recall. Percept. Mot. Skills 84, 1307–1313. doi: 10.2466/pms.1997.84.3c.1307

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parente, J. A. (1976). Music preference as a factor of music distraction. Percept. Mot. Skills 43, 337–338. doi: 10.2466/pms.1976.43.1.337

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pool, M. M., Koolstra, C. M., and Voort, T. H. A. (2003). The impact of background radio and television on high school students' homework performance. J. Commun. 53, 74–87. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-2466.2003.tb03006.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rangaswamy, M., Porjesz, B., Chorlian, D. B., Wang, K., Jones, K. A., Bauer, L. O., et al. (2002). Beta power in the EEG of alcoholics. Biol. Psychiatry 52, 831–842. doi: 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01362-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ransdell, S. E., and Gilroy, L. (2001). The effects of background music on word processed writing. Comput. Hum. Behav. 17, 141–148. doi: 10.1016/S0747-5632(00)00043-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ray, W. J., and Cole, H. W. (1985). EEG alpha activity reflects attentional demands, and beta activity reflects emotional and cognitive processes. Science 228, 750–752.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Schellenberg, E. G., Nakata, T., Hunter, P. G., and Tamoto, S. (2007). Exposure to music and cognitive performance: tests of children and adults. Psychol. Music 35, 5–19. doi: 10.1177/0305735607068885

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schlittmeier, S. J., and Hellbrück, J. (2009). Background music as noise abatement in open-plan offices: a laboratory study on performance effects and subjective preferences. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 23, 684–697. doi: 10.1002/acp.1498

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmidtke, J. I., and Heller, W. (2004). Personality, affect and EEG: predicting patterns of regional brain activity related to extraversion and neuroticism. Pers. Individ. Dif. 36, 717–732. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00129-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schreiber, E. H. (1988). Influence of music on college students' achievement. Percept. Mot. Skills 66, 338–338. doi: 10.2466/pms.1988.66.1.338

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, B. D., Kline, R., Lindgren, K., Ferro, M., Smith, D. A., and Nespor, A. (1995). The lateralized processing of affect in emotionally labile extraverts and introverts: central and autonomic effects. Biol. Psychol. 39, 143–157. doi: 10.1016/0301-0511(94)00968-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, W. F., Schellenberg, E. G., and Husain, G. (2001). Arousal, mood, and the Mozart effect. Psychol. Sci. 12, 248–251. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thompson, W. F., Schellenberg, E. G., and Letnic, A. K. (2012). Fast and loud background music disrupts reading comprehension. Psychol. Music 40, 700–708. doi: 10.1177/0305735611400173

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woo, E. W., and Kanachi, M. (2005). The effects of music type and volume on short-term memory. Tohoku Psychol. Folia 64, 68–76. Available online at: https://tohoku.repo.nii.ac.jp/?action=pages_view_main&active_action=repository_view_main_item_detail&item_id=1462&item_no=1&page_id=33&block_id=38

Yerkes, R. M., and Dodson, J. D. (1908). The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. J. Comp. Neurol. Psychol. 18, 459–482. doi: 10.1002/cne.920180503

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Eysenck's theory of personality, background music, extraversion, cognitive task performance, EEG, alpha/beta band power, arousal, focused listening

Citation: Küssner MB (2017) Eysenck's Theory of Personality and the Role of Background Music in Cognitive Task Performance: A Mini-Review of Conflicting Findings and a New Perspective. Front. Psychol. 8:1991. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01991

Received: 11 August 2017; Accepted: 31 October 2017;
Published: 14 November 2017.

Edited by:

Gary Edward McPherson, University of Melbourne, Australia

Reviewed by:

Adina Mornell, Hochschule für Musik und Theater München, Germany
Adrian Furnham, University College London, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2017 Küssner. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Mats B. Küssner, mats.kussner@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.