Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 09 February 2018
Sec. Developmental Psychology

A Chip Off the Old Block: Parents’ Subtle Ethnic Prejudice Predicts Children’s Implicit Prejudice

  • 1Department of Dynamic and Clinical Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy
  • 2Department of Education, Roma Tre University, Rome, Italy

The increasing flow of immigrants in many European countries and the growing presence of children from immigrant families in schools makes it relevant to study the development of prejudice in children. Parents play an important role in shaping children’s values and their attitudes toward members of other ethnic groups; an intergenerational transmission of prejudice has been found in a number of studies targeting adolescents. The present study aims to investigate the intergenerational transmission of ethnic prejudice in 3- to 9- year-old children and its relations to parenting styles. Parents’ blatant and subtle ethnic prejudice and parenting style are measured together with children’s explicit and implicit ethnic prejudice in pupils and parents of preschool and primary schools in the region of Rome, Italy (N = 318). Results show that parents’ subtle prejudice predicts children’s implicit prejudice regardless of the parenting style. Findings indicate that children might acquire prejudice by means of the parents’ implicit cognition and automatic behavior and educational actions. Implications for future studies and insights for possible applied interventions are discussed.

Introduction

The multiethnic character of increasingly large parts of urban and rural communities in western societies makes the topic of interethnic contact more and more relevant in order to improve the quality of life and personal relations in daily life contexts, and to foster social inclusion. Prejudice reduction in adults and children is a crucial issue in this process (Passiatore et al., 2017). The dynamics of prejudice formation and expression in adults was investigated quite in depth in the field of social psychology and social sciences in general (Aboud and Steele, 2017). While the presence of ethnic prejudice is manifest in children as young as 3 years old in terms of ingroup favoritism and outgroup discrimination (Dweck, 2009), the identification of the environmental sources of ethnic prejudice in children still needs to be studied.

Long before they are able to identify themselves in relation to a social group, infants in their first year of life show early capacities of social discrimination such as a clearer preference for faces of their same-ethnicity (Kelly et al., 2007), for someone speaking their language and for toys selected by someone speaking their own language (Kinzler et al., 2007). The same pattern of results can be observed regarding the children’s preference for familiar people and social stimuli. An ethnicity bias clearly emerges in children as young as 3 or 4 years of age (Aboud, 1988; Augoustinos and Rosewarne, 2001; Bigler and Liben, 2006; Aboud, 2008; Gaither et al., 2014), it appears to reach its peak in middle childhood around 7–8 years old, and then it gradually declines. This trend does not apply to all types of children’s racial or ethnic attitudes, differentiating instead between explicit and implicit attitudes: the former are conscious and under intentional control while the latter are automatic and relatively out of attentional control (see for example, Dunham et al., 2013; Baron, 2015). These attitudes do not correlate with each other: on the one hand, explicit attitudes grow until middle childhood and then progressively decline; on the other hand, implicit ethnic attitudes seem to be relatively stable over the time (e.g., Rutland, 1999; Degner and Wentura, 2010). For example, 6 years old children are equally biased for both implicit and explicit levels (Baron and Banaji, 2006), while 10 years old children show a dissociation between the two measures (i.e., the level of implicit ethnic attitudes stays constant while self-reported preferences for the own ingroup decrease). This process is likely due to the increased effect of social norms prohibiting the explicit expression of negative ethnic attitudes. Such social norms might inhibit the overt expression of these attitudes without reducing the bias that becomes observable only in implicit tasks. The activation of automatic prejudice and the expression of explicit prejudice not only follow different developmental paths but also use two distinct modes of processing (Moors and De Houwer, 2006). Broadly speaking, automatic processes operate regardless of attentional resources and they do not require intention as opposed to the non-automatic processes that, differently, are subject to the person’s awareness and require more attention and cognitive effort. Measures of explicit attitudes reflect deliberate and controlled responses that can be altered or suppressed, whereas implicit measures tap automatic and spontaneous processes that are difficult to consciously modify or extinguish (Schneider and Shiffrin, 1977). Associative processes give rise to implicit attitudes independently from subjective truth or falsity (Gawronski and Bodenhausen, 2006), and in turn, they may predict discriminatory behaviors (Gawronski and Bodenhausen, 2007), and affect social judgments and interpersonal relations (Dovidio et al., 2002). Therefore, the development and expression of implicit ethnic attitudes in children is a worth issue for developmental and educational studies. In particular it is relevant to better clarify the role of different socializing agents, such as parents and the education system, in the development and transmission of prejudice in childhood. Based on these assumptions, the goal of this study consists of investigating the expression of children explicit and implicit prejudice and the prejudice transmission process from parents to children.

The Transmission of Ethnic Attitudes and Prejudice

The development of intergroup attitudes is conceivable as the result of the interaction between genetic predispositions, socialization influences, and situational determinants (Hatemi et al., 2009; Verhust and Hatemi, 2013). Children’s attitudes crucially depend on early socialization experiences and therefore are influenced by significant adults (Carlson and Iovini, 1985; Bigler and Liben, 2007; McGlothlin and Killen, 2010). Socializing agents play a role in this process, suggesting children’s intergroup attitudes to be a function of the attitudes of their parents through the process of social transmission (Radke-Yarrow et al., 1952; Allport, 1954; Bandura, 1977; Aboud and Amato, 2001; Nesdale and Flesser, 2001). Regarding ethnic prejudice, a number of researches confirm a strong similarity between the parents’ and their children’s racial attitudes (O’Bryan et al., 2004; Jaspers et al., 2008; Rodriguez-Garcia and Wagner, 2009; Dhont and Van Hiel, 2012; Dhont et al., 2013; Meeusen, 2014), while other studies find only a limited correspondence (Aboud and Doyle, 1996; Hello et al., 2004; Vittrup and Holden, 2011). A recent meta-analysis highlights that children’s and parents’ intergroup attitudes are related with each other, showing ranges from small to moderate effect sizes (Degner and Dalege, 2013). According to this meta-analysis, a relevant factor in such effect sizes’ variability is the direct vs. indirect measurement of parent’s prejudice, as indirect assessments (e.g., asking children to assess their parent’s prejudice) might lead to inflated estimates of children–parent correlations. Thus, in our study, we relied only on direct assessments of both children and parents prejudice.

Furthermore, intergenerational transmission may be stronger when the significant adults engender a close relationship with their children (Sinclair et al., 2005b) or when children are highly identified with their significant adults (Sinclair et al., 2005a). These findings lead to some considerations about the nature of the mechanisms regulating intergenerational transmission. Studies on the transmission of values between parents and adolescents indicate that the age of the child and the content of the values or attitudes may play a role, together with the authoritativeness of the parents’ educational style (e.g., Pinquart and Silbereisen, 2004). If the role of parents, and of the quality of the parent-child relationship, for children development and adjustment is unequivocally clear (Stevenson et al., 1990; Rodriguez-Garcia and Wagner, 2009), on the other hand, the relationship between specific parenting styles and the child’s outcomes in terms of competence, self-confidence and emotional control are controversial. Some studies indicate the authoritative parenting style as the best for children well-being and achievement (Pinquart, 2016, 2017) and others collect evidences of a more multifaceted phenomenon where the children’s outcomes cannot be predicted by a specific parenting style (García and Gracia, 2009).

Therefore, it is interesting to clarify the relationship between parenting style and prejudice transmission. It has been suggested that parenting variables, such as the perceived support, affect the parents’ prejudice influence on children attitudes (Miklikowska, 2016). Some studies considered individual factors such as, for instance, authoritarianism, social dominance, intrinsic vs. extrinsic goals, or external vs. internal orientation in prejudice formation and expression (e.g., Duriez et al., 2008; Sibley and Duckitt, 2008; Duriez, 2011). However, studies explicitly considering the effect of specific parenting style on children ethnic prejudice and including a direct measure of parenting style are still lacking, and would shed light on the role of educational practices on prejudice development.

Thus, in the present study we measured the influence of parenting style construct articulated in three different forms, authoritative, authoritarian, permissive on the relationship between parents and children ethnic prejudice.

Another important aspect that did not received sufficient consideration in previous studies on prejudice’s transmission is the distinction between explicit or blatant and implicit or latent forms of prejudice. It is arguable that both can be influenced by parents’ attitudes and practices in different ways.

At the explicit level, children tend to imitate and conform to the explicit attitudes and behaviors of their parents. Also for implicit ethnic attitudes, a number of studies report a similarity between children and adults’ patterns of prejudice (e.g., Sinclair et al., 2005a; Dunham et al., 2006; Castelli et al., 2009; Vezzali et al., 2012). These findings, however, are related mostly to adolescent or children above the age of 10. The finding that implicit attitudes are acquired within the family and that they are probably linked to early socialization experiences, needs to be confirmed in relation to preschool or primary school years. Furthermore, it is still not clear if parenting practices are responsible for the transmission of attitudes, or if children conform to their parents’ behaviors, and which is the role of the other environmental factors in this process.

The Present Study

The general purpose of the current study is to fill in the existing gaps in the previous literature on internal and family-agents influencing the explicit and implicit ethnic prejudice transmission in children, by expanding and generalizing the findings of previous studies.

A previous work by Castelli et al. (2009) found for example that implicit prejudices of mothers, predicts the racial attitudes of their preschool children; these authors however, only use an explicit measure. Thus, in the present study, we assess children’s implicit ethnic attitudes through an implicit measure, such as the Implicit Association Test.

Other few studies analyzed the intergenerational transmission of implicit attitudes from parents to their children but they only include pre-adolescents (e.g., Sinclair et al., 2005a). Thus, in the present study, we consider preschool and primary school children (3 to 9 years old).

Also, as previously mentioned, we want to take into account also the role of parenting styles on the ethnic prejudice transmission, which was underinvestigated by previous studies.

In sum, our study aims to analyze: (a) the role of the parents’ blatant and subtle prejudice on their children explicit and implicit prejudice; (b) the role of parenting styles in the prejudice transmission from parents to children; (c) the role of children age in the development and expression of children explicit and implicit ethnic prejudice. Coherently with all the literature reviewed so far (e.g., Baron, 2015), we expect that:

(1)   the parents’ subtle ethnic prejudice would predict the child’s implicit prejudice, also after controlling for the parenting style (H1);

(2)   the parents’ blatant ethnic prejudice would correlate with the child’s explicit ethnic prejudice (H2);

(3)   significant differences in children prejudice across age groups would be present only in the case of explicit prejudice (H3).

Materials and Methods

Participants

A total of 318 children and one parent for each of them participated to the study, in the context of a larger EU-funded research and intervention project (The SOFT-School and family together for the integration of immigrant children project, funded in 2012–2015 by the LLP-KA2MP program). The study protocol was approved by the directors of the involved schools. Parents were asked to voluntary participate to the study and expressed their informed consent to the involvement of their children in the study. The children attended 11 preschool and primary schools in the cities of Rome and Fondi, Latium region, Italy. They are all Caucasian children with Italian nationality and ranged in age from 3 to 9 years (M = 6.06; SD = 1.86). Girls constituted 46% of the sample. The involved schools were located in neighborhoods with similar socio economic conditions (e.g., low-middle class), and the involved classrooms were characterized for similar proportions of pupils with an immigrant background.

Procedure and Instruments

Parents

Parents received at home the questionnaire and one parent per family completed it. All questionnaires were completed by mothers. The questionnaire included two scales:

1.   the Italian adaptation (Arcuri and Boca, 1996) of the questionnaire by Pettigrew and Meertens (1995). The scale is composed of 20 items measuring ethnic prejudice on a five-point Likert scale (1 = totally disagree; 5 = totally agree). Ten items measure the blatant prejudice (e.g., “Immigrants take jobs that should be up to Italians”) and 10 measure the subtle prejudice (e.g., “Immigrants that live in our country transmit to their children values and abilities not required in Italy”). The total prejudice is the sum of blatant and subtle prejudice. High scores correspond to high prejudice levels.

2.   Parenting was measured by a 32 items version of the “Parent Style and Dimension Questionnaire” (Robinson et al., 2001) that describes three parenting profiles: authoritative (e.g., “I am responsive to my child’s feelings and needs”), authoritarian (e.g., “I use criticism to make my child improve his/her behavior”), and permissive (e.g., “I find it difficult to discipline my child”). The scale was back translated in Italian. Scores range on a five-point scale (1 = never; 5 = always).

Children

Children were tested individually at school. For measuring their explicit ethnic prejudice, they were shown six photos of children (six boys or six girls according to the gender of the participant child) selected from six different ethnic groups (Caucasian, African, Asiatic Indians, Asiatic Chinese, Arabian, South-American; Lo Coco et al., 2000). The ethnic groups were chosen according to the composition of immigrant population in the area of the research. The six photos were positioned in front of the child who are required to indicate: (1) the child that he/she would like to be; (2) the two children that he/she would like to invite at home to spend some time together. Next the experimenter asked to the child to assign adjectives and verbs written on a card and read by the experimenter to each child: seven positive adjectives (nice; good; clever; clean; likeable; happy; obedient), seven negative adjectives (bad, nasty, stupid, dirty, unpleasant, unhappy, disobedient), three positive verbs (give, hug, help), three negative verbs (steal, beat, disturb), and three neutral verbs (watch the TV, eat, sleep). We assigned score “1” when the student chose a child who belong to his/her ethnicity. We assigned “0” when the student chose a child who doesn’t belong to his/her ethnicity. The global score of explicit ethnic prejudice is composed by the sum of scores at items 1 and 2 and the score at the positive and negative adjectives and verbs (the score “0” of negative adjectives and verbs was converted in “1”). Therefore, the total score in this variable could range from 0 (low explicit prejudice) to 22 (high explicit prejudice).

Children’s implicit ethnic prejudice was assessed by the child version of the IAT (Child-IAT) adapted to comply with the recommendations by Baron and Banaji (2006) and designed using Super Lab Pro 4. The test consisted of seven blocks (Nosek et al., 2005). In the first block the students were asked to categorize photographs of Caucasian, African, Asiatic Indians, Asiatic Chinese, Arabian, South-American children (the photographs employed are different from explicit prejudice test) in two different groups – Caucasian and Other – represented by two pictures (Caucasian children group and Multi-ethnic group). In the second block, they categorized verbal stimuli by positive (nice, good, happy, boring, clear) or negative valence (bad, nasty, unhappy, funny, dirty) in two different groups represented by two faces: a green and happy face for the positive stimuli and a red and unhappy face for negative stimuli. The verbal stimuli are audio-recorded to allow pre-school children to participate and to avoid interferences of the different reading abilities of the primary school children with the test’s performance. The next two blocks were combined categorization tasks: respondents were asked to press the left (blue) button when a stimulus in either the “Caucasian” category or “Positive” category appeared and the right (yellow) button when a stimulus in the “Other” or “Bad” category appeared. The third block was a practice block while the fourth block was “critical” block. The fifth block consisted of another simple categorization task, however, with the reversed positioning of “Caucasian” and “Other.” This was done to avoid the influence of previously learned spatial positions of category names (Schnabel et al., 2007). Blocks 6 and 7 were analogous to blocks 3 and 4, with the opposite pairing of the target and attribute categories. Each stimulus was presented until the child provided a response, after which an inter-stimuli period of 150 ms followed. Incorrect answers were followed by red symbol “X” in the center of the screen. The child can’t move on until the correct answer is given. The data were filtered excluding those with latency higher than 10000 ms and with correct answers’ mean inferior to 0.80. The wrong answers were replaced by correct answers’ mean penalized with 600 ms. The Cronbach Alpha is 0.84.

Statistical Analysis

To investigate the relationships involving parents’ ethnic prejudice and children’s ethnic prejudice, we computed zero-order correlations among these variables (the 0.05 level of significance was adopted throughout all analyses). Descriptive statistics of our sample are also provided. To test our main hypotheses (H1 and H2), we carried out two multiple regressions, in which parenting styles and parents’ ethnic prejudice (blatant and subtle) were entered as predictors of children’s implicit and explicit ethnic prejudice, respectively. To test our hypothesis on the different effects of age on explicit vs. implicit prejudice (H3), we created three different age groups (according to previous literature on ethnic prejudice development, e.g., Raabe and Beelmann, 2011): preschool (3–5 years old); first class of Primary school (6–7 years old); fourth class of Primary school (9 years old). Then we tested age-group differences in explicit and implicit prejudice through two univariate ANOVAs.

Results

Descriptive statistics and bivariate correlations linking parents’ and children’s ethnic prejudice measures are displayed in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Means, standard deviations, bivariate correlations, and Cohen’s d.

As predicted, children’s implicit ethnic prejudice is significantly and positively related to parents’ subtle prejudice. No significant correlation emerges between parents’ blatant prejudice and children either explicit or implicit prejudice. Concerning parenting styles, no relations emerged with children’s prejudice. However, as arguable, parent’s prejudice (especially blatant prejudice) is positively correlated with an authoritarian parenting style, and negatively correlated with an authoritative parenting style. Also, interestingly, children’s implicit and explicit prejudice are not significantly associated each other, in line with previous literature. On the contrary, parent’s blatant and subtle prejudice scores are significantly and positively correlated.

In line with our main hypothesis (H1), a multiple regression analysis showed that parents’ subtle prejudice has a significant positive effect on children’s implicit ethnic prejudice (β = 0.21; p = 0.04), also after controlling for parenting styles and parent’s blatant prejudice. No significant effects of parenting styles and parent’s blatant prejudice on children implicit ethnic prejudice were detected (p > 0.1).

The multiple regression conducted testing our second hypothesis showed no significant effects of parents’ blatant and subtle prejudice and parenting styles on children’s explicit prejudice. Scatterplot graphs for the parent-child prejudice links were also run (subtle-implicit and blatant-explicit, respectively); these graphs are displayed in Figures 1, 2, and show that the significant and non-significant effects we detected are not likely to be attributed to a few outliers.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Scatterplot of relations between parents subtle prejudice and children implicit prejudice (IAT).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Scatterplot of relations between parents blatant prejudice and children explicit prejudice.

Children’s prejudice scores across the different age groups are showed in Table 2. As predicted in our second hypothesis, univariate ANOVAs confirms the significant variation of explicit ethnic prejudice across age (F(2,341) = 4.13, p = 0.017). The ethnic explicit prejudice significantly increases from preschool age (3–5 years) to middle childhood (6–7 years) followed by a decrease in the late childhood (9 years). Duncan post hoc comparison revealed indeed that explicit prejudice scores in 6–7 years old children were significantly higher compared to both 3–5 and 9 years old children (p < 0.05); these, in turn, did not significantly differed each other (p = 0.96). Conversely, as expected, no significant variations of implicit prejudice emerged across the three age groups (F(2,198) = 0.686, p = 0.505).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Means and standard deviations in children explicit and implicit ethnic prejudice across age groups.

Discussion

The purpose of the present study was to investigate the role of parental influences in the development and expression of ethnic prejudice in preschool and primary school children, in particular taking into account the distinction between different forms (i.e., explicit vs. implicit) of prejudice in adults and children.

While it is widely acknowledged that the social environment and relevant socialization agents (such as parents) certainly play a role in shaping relevant social attitudes in children and adolescents, researches so far have described the nature and entity of these relations in inconsistent ways. By the use of explicit and implicit prejudice measures in children (and blatant vs. subtle prejudice measures in their parents) we wanted to ascertain the process of intergenerational transmission of ethnic prejudice.

Our data are coherent with previous findings (e.g., Castelli et al., 2009) showing that parental blatant and subtle prejudice are differently linked to young children’s prejudice. In fact, while children’s explicit prejudice seems unrelated to their parents either blatant or subtle prejudice, it is not surprising that children’s implicit ethnic prejudice is positively predicted by parents’ level of subtle ethnic prejudice, given the extensive amount of empirical evidence on dual process accounts of human cognition, affect, decision making, and behavior (Bargh and Chartrand, 1999; Kahneman, 2002; Strack and Deutsch, 2004). However, our findings do not contradict the existing literature on the importance of imitation process in child–adult relations (e.g., Koffka, 1935; Piaget, 1946; Bandura, 1977). Our findings suggest that children’s prejudice may be rooted in the automatic behavior and implicit social influence processes enacted by their significant adults, more than in what parents explicitly think (and likely say) about ethnically different people to their children. This findings also strengthen the idea that implicit and explicit attitudes are activated and expressed through different channels and processes (Schneider and Shiffrin, 1977; Moors and De Houwer, 2006). Children’s explicit prejudice is under their intentional control (and therefore might be mitigated by social norms that discourage the overt expression of negative ethnic attitudes) more than under the influence of their parents’ blatant attitudes. On the contrary, the children’s implicit attitudes, being out of their intentional control, might be influenced by the subtle parents’ prejudice. In sum, the children’s life environment, including parent’s attitudes, do play a role, but the different facets of implicit and explicit social cognition should be considered to fully account for prejudice development in childhood (e.g., Baron and Banaji, 2006; Castelli et al., 2009).

Finally, we have not found clear evidence for a direct role of parenting styles in the intergenerational transmission of ethnic prejudice: the parents’ subtle prejudice levels have a role in predicting the children implicit prejudice independently of the specific parenting style put in practice by parents. Nevertheless, since our data show that an authoritarian parenting style is positively linked to parent’s prejudice (both blatant and subtle), while an authoritative style is negatively linked to parent’s blatant prejudice, it might well be that parenting styles exert an indirect influence on parental prejudice transmission, particularly at an implicit level.

The present findings also confirm our general assumption that implicit and explicit ethnic prejudice in children are distinct phenomena, and might follow different developmental patterns. In fact, implicit prejudice scores in our sample are relatively constant in children ranging from 3 to 9 years old while explicit prejudice in 6–7 years-old children show higher scores than in 3–5 and 9 years-old children.

Some limitations must however be acknowledged in our study. First, we measured implicit prejudice in parents by a self-report measure, such as the Pettigrew and Meertens questionnaire (Pettigrew, 1991; Pettigrew and Meertens, 1995; Meertens and Pettigrew, 1997). Although this scale is acknowledged as the most referential tool to measure the two types of prejudice, the use of self-report measures for implicit constructs may be questionable, since answering requires a certain degree of the respondents’ awareness about their attitude toward the item’s content (Hoffman et al., 2005). The fact that both explicit and implicit prejudice of the parents were measured with questionnaires (e.g., Castelli et al., 2009) might not allow to draw definitive conclusions about the true implicit prejudice of the parents. For example, is it possible that the highly correlated parents’ subtle and blatant prejudice scores are due to the fact that they were both measured with a questionnaire. Furthermore, it should be considered that the statements in parents’ questionnaires are about immigrants versus native Italians, while in children’s explicit prejudice measures children are asked to judge groups that differ not only on ethnicity, but also on language and culture, which is something more complex than just “us versus other.” Future studies, therefore, could extend and confirm our findings by means of actual implicit measures for both parents and children.

Also, the lack of influence of parenting on the intergenerational transmission of ethnic prejudice in our findings might be the effect of the instrument we used to measure parenting style. In fact, especially for the assessment of authoritarian parenting style, the formulation of the items could have discourage parents to provide reliable answers because of social desirability (e.g., “I spank my child when I don’t like what he/she does or says”).

Finally, it is worth to point out that the present findings might have relevance from an applied point of view. The role of parents in shaping the socialization experiences of their children is well-established in the framework of ecological development (Bronfenbrenner, 1979) and by studies on interethnic relationships in adolescents and children (Bifulco et al., 2009; Edmonds and Killen, 2009; Munniksmaa et al., 2012). Because of the increasing multiethnic character of daily life environments in many current human societies (Vertovec, 2007), interethnic relations and inclusiveness is an important issue for children and adolescent in schools (e.g., Thijs and Verkuyten, 2014; see also Graham and Cohen, 1997; Pirchio et al., 2015). It is widely acknowledged that the quality of interethnic relationships may contribute to academic achievement and well-being for both majority and minority groups students (Driessen, 2002; Van Ewijk and Sleegers, 2010; Thijs et al., 2014).

Knowing that the parents’ influence on children ethnic attitudes can follow also an implicit and automatic path may offer two important indications for educational and psychological intervention programs aiming at reducing ethnic prejudices in children, and therefore fostering positive interethnic relationships. The first one relates to the need of targeting not only children, but also parents for reducing the children’s prejudice. This might help to avoiding feelings of threat and incoherence in children who eventually would be faced with interventions that propose attitudes and values that are in contrast with parents’ explicit or implicit attitudes. The second one is related to the importance of addressing not only the explicit and aware side of ethnic prejudice, but also tackling the latent and implicit attitudes which the parents might not be aware of, but which are able of activating and reinforcing children’s prejudice (Patterson and Bigler, 2006). In this direction, interventions based on the Contact Hypothesis theory (Kawakami et al., 2007; Thijs and Verkuyten, 2014) and involving the children together with their significant adult (Pirchio et al., 2017a,b) could give positive outcomes.

Conclusion

Our findings offer an important contribution to plan and design effective social and educational intervention programs for prejudice reduction involving parents as intervention targets for effective attitudes’ changes in children. Exploring the possible synergies between different individual and contextual factors at the basis of a more socially inclusive and sustainable human experience in both education and daily life contexts involving human relations to the surrounding environment (e.g., Carrus et al., 2015, 2017; Mercado-Domeìnech et al., 2017; Panno et al., 2017) is also a worthy issue for future research investigating the effectiveness of social inclusion interventions programs in schools.

Ethics Statement

Adult participants to this study expressed their oral consent to take part to the survey, and voluntary accepted to fill in a self-reported paper and pencil questionnaire. For children participants, parents gave written informed consent to participation. This study did not require approval from an ethical committee according to Italian regulations.

Author Contributions

SP and YP equally contributed to writing the text (shared first authorship); AP, GC, YP, and SP equally contributed to conducting the statistical analysis and revising the text; YP collected the data; FM contributed to implement the child IAT procedure (defining the script, data coding, and analyzing).

Funding

The present study is part of the project “School and Family Together for the Integration of Immigrant Children” (SOFT), that received a European Union funding in the EACEA (Grant agreement no. 2012-4479/001/001) LLP-KA2MP program, 2012–2015.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Aboud, F. E. (1988). Children and Prejudice. London, England: Blackwell.

Google Scholar

Aboud, F. E. (2008). “A social-cognitive developmental theory of prejudice,” in The Handbook of Race, Racism, and the Developing Child, eds S. M. Quintana and C. McKown (Hoboken, NJ: Wiley), 55–71.

Google Scholar

Aboud, F. E., and Amato, M. (2001). “Developmental and socialization influences on intergroup bias,” in Blackwell’s Handbook of Social Psychology: Intergroup Processes, eds R. Brown and S. Gaertner (Malden, MA: Blackwell), 65–85.

Google Scholar

Aboud, F. E., and Doyle, A. (1996). Does talk of race foster prejudice or tolerance in children? Can. J. Behav. Sci. 28, 161–170. doi: 10.1037/0008-400X.28.3.161

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aboud, F. E., and Steele, J. R. (2017). “Theoretical perspectives on the development of implicit and explicit prejudice,” in The Wiley Handbook of Group Processes in Children and Adolescents, eds A. Rutland, D. Nesdale and C. S. Brown (Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons), 167.

Google Scholar

Allport, G. (1954). The Nature of Prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison–Wesley.

Google Scholar

Arcuri, L., and Boca, S. (1996). “Pregiudizio e affiliazione politica: destra e sinistra di fronte all’immigrazione dal Terzo Mondo,” in Psicologia e Politica, eds P. Legrenzi and V. Girotto (Milano: Raffaello Cortina Editore), 241–274.

Google Scholar

Augoustinos, M., and Rosewarne, D. L. (2001). Stereotype knowledge and prejudice in children. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 19, 143–156. doi: 10.1348/026151001165912

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1977). Social Learning Theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Google Scholar

Bargh, J. A., and Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being. Am. Psychol. 54, 462–479. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.462

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron, A. S. (2015). Constraints on the development of implicit intergroup attitudes. Child Dev. Perspect. 9, 50–54. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12105

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baron, A. S., and Banaji, M. R. (2006). The development of implicit attitudes: evidence of race evaluations from ages 6, 10 and adulthood. Psychol. Sci. 17, 53–58. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2005.01664.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bifulco, R., Ladd, H. F., and Ross, S. (2009). Public school choice and integration: evidence from Durham, North Carolina. Soc. Sci. Res. 38, 71–85. doi: 10.1016/j.ssresearch.2008.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bigler, R. S., and Liben, L. S. (2006). “A developmental intergroup theory of social stereotypes and prejudice,” in Advances in Child Development and Behavior, Vol. 34, ed. R. V. Kail (San Diego, CA: Elsevier), 39–89.

Google Scholar

Bigler, R. S., and Liben, L. S. (2007). Developmental intergroup theory: explaining and reducing children’s social stereotyping and prejudice. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 16, 162–166. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00496.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The Ecology of Human Development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Google Scholar

Carlson, J. M., and Iovini, J. (1985). The transmission of racial attitudes from fathers to sons: a study of blacks and whites. Adolescence 20, 233–237.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Carrus, G., Passiatore, Y., Pirchio, S., and Scopelliti, M. (2015). Contact with nature in educational settings might help cognitive functioning and promote positive social behavior. Psyecology 6, 191–212. doi: 10.1080/21711976.2015.1026079

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carrus, G., Scopelliti, M., Panno, A., Lafortezza, R., Colangelo, G., Pirchio, S., et al. (2017). A different way to stay in touch with ‘urban nature’: the perceived restorative qualities of botanical gardens. Front. Psychol. 8:914. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00914

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castelli, L., Zogmaister, C., and Tomelleri, S. (2009). The transmission of racial attitudes within the family. Dev. Psychol. 45, 586–591. doi: 10.1037/a0014619

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Degner, J., and Dalege, J. (2013). The apple does not fall far from the tree, or does it? A meta- analysis of parent-child similarity in intergroup attitudes. Psychol. Bull. 139, 1270–1304. doi: 10.1037/a0031436

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Degner, J., and Wentura, D. (2010). Automatic prejudice in childhood and early adolescence. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 98, 356–374. doi: 10.1037/a0017993

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dhont, K., Roets, A., and Van Hiel, A. (2013). The intergenerational transmission of need for closure underlies the transmission of authoritarianism and anti-immigrant prejudice. Pers. Ind. Diff. 54, 779–784. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.12.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dhont, K., and Van Hiel, A. (2012). Intergroup contact buffers against the intergenerational transmission of authoritarianism and racial prejudice. J. Res. Pers. 46, 231–234. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2011.12.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dovidio, J. F., Kawakami, K., and Gaertner, S. L. (2002). Implicit and explicit prejudice and interracial interaction. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 82, 62–68. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.82.1.62

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Driessen, G. (2002). School composition and achievement in primary education: a large-scale multilevel approach. Stud. Educ. Evaluat. 28, 347–368. doi: 10.1016/S0191-491X(02)00043-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., and Banaji, M. R. (2006). From American city to Japanese village: a cross-cultural investigation of implicit race attitudes. Child Dev. 77, 1268–1281. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.2006.00933.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunham, Y., Chen, E. E., and Banaji, M. R. (2013). Two signatures of implicit intergroup attitudes: developmental invariance and early enculturation. Psychol. Sci. 24, 860–868. doi: 10.1177/0956797612463081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duriez, B. (2011). Adolescent ethnic prejudice: understanding the effects of parental extrinsic versus intrinsic goal promotion. J. Soc. Psychol. 151, 441–454. doi: 10.1080/00224545.2010.490571

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duriez, B., Soenens, B., and Vansteenkiste, M. (2008). The intergenerational transmission of authoritarianism: the mediating role of parental goal promotion. J. Res. Pers. 42, 622–642. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2007.08.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dweck, C. S. (2009). Prejudice: how it develops and how it can be undone. Hum. Dev. 52, 371–376. doi: 10.1159/000242351

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Edmonds, C., and Killen, M. (2009). Do adolescents’ perceptions of parental racial attitudes relate to their outgroup contact and cross-race relationships? Group Processes Intergroup Relat. 12, 5–21. doi: 10.1177/1368430208098773

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gaither, S. E., Schultz, J. R., Pauker, K., Sommers, S. R., Maddox, K. B., and Ambady, N. (2014). Essentialist thinking predicts decrements in children’s memory for racially ambiguous faces. Dev. Psychol. 50, 482–488. doi: 10.1037/a0033493

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

García, F., and Gracia, E. (2009). Is always authoritative the optimum parenting style? Evidence from Spanish families. Adolescence 44, 101–131.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Gawronski, B., and Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: an integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychol. Bull. 132, 692–731. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.692

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gawronski, B., and Bodenhausen, G. V. (2007). Unraveling the processes underlying evaluation: attitudes from the perspective of the APE model. Soc. Cogn. 25, 687–717. doi: 10.1521/soco.2007.25.5.687

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graham, J. A., and Cohen, R. (1997). Race and sex as factors in children’s sociometric ratings and friendship choices. Soc. Dev. 6, 353–370. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.1997.tb00111.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hatemi, P. K., Funk, C. L., Medlad, S. E., Maes, H. M., Silberg, J. L., Martin, N. G., et al. (2009). Genetic and environmental transmission of political attitudes over a life time. J. Polit. 3, 1141–1156. doi: 10.1017/S0022381609090938

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hello, E., Scheepers, P., Vermulst, A., and Gerris, J. R. M. (2004). Association between educational attainment and ethnic distance in young adults: socialization by schools or parents? Acta Sociol. 47, 253–275. doi: 10.1177/0001699304046222

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hoffman, W., Gawronski, B., Gschwendner, T., Le, H., and Schmitt, M. (2005). A meta-analysis on the correlation between the implicit association test and explicit self-report measures. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 31, 1369–1385. doi: 10.1177/0146167205275613

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaspers, E., Lubbers, M., and De Vries, J. (2008). Parents, children and the distance between them: long term socialization effects in the Netherlands. J. Compar. Fam. Stud. 39, 39–58. doi: 10.1177/1469540516648372

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kahneman, D. (2002). Maps of bounded rationality: a perspective on intuitive judgment and choice. Nobel Prize Lecture 8, 351–401.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kawakami, K., Phills, C. E., Steele, J. R., and Dovidio, J. F. (2007). (Close) distance makes the heart grow fonder: improving implicit racial attitudes and interracial interactions through approach behaviors. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 40, 414–431. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.92.6.957

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, D. J., Liu, S., Ge, L., Quinn, P. C., Slater, A. M., Lee, K., et al. (2007). Cross-race preferences for same-race faces extend beyond the African versus Caucasian contrast in 3-month-old infants. Infancy 11, 87–95. doi: 10.1080/15250000709336871

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kinzler, K. D., Dupoux, E., and Spelke, E. S. (2007). The native language of social cognition. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 104, 12577–12580. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0705345104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koffka, K. (1935). Principles of Gestalt psychology. New York, NY: Harcourt Brace.

Google Scholar

Lo Coco, A., Pace, U., and Zappulla, C. (2000). I precursori dell’identità etnica in età infantile. Età Evolut. 66, 77–85.

Google Scholar

McGlothlin, H., and Killen, M. (2010). How social experience is related to children’s intergroup attitudes. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 40, 625–634. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.733

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meertens, R. W., and Pettigrew, T. F. (1997). Is subtle prejudice really prejudice? Public Opin. Q. 61, 54–71. doi: 10.1086/297786

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meeusen, C. (2014). The parent–child similarity in cross-group friendship and anti-immigrant prejudice: a study among 15-year old adolescents and both their parents in Belgium. J. Res. Pers. 50, 46–55. doi: 10.1002/per.2011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mercado-Domeìnech, S. J., Carrus, G., Teraìn-Aìlvarez- Del-Rey, A., and Pirchio, S. (2017). Valuation theory: an environmental, developmental and evolutionary psychological approach. Implications for the field of environmental education [Teoria della valutazione: un approccio psicologico ambientale, evolutivo ed evoluzionistico. Implicazioni in campo educativo]. J. Educ. Cult. Psychol. Stud. 16, 77–97. doi: 10.7358/ecps-2017-016-merc

CrossRef Full Text

Miklikowska, M. (2016). Like parent, like child? Development of prejudice and tolerance towards immigrants. Br. J. Psychol. 107, 95–116. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moors, A., and De Houwer, J. (2006). Automaticity: a conceptual and theoretical analysis. Psychol. Bull. 132, 297–326. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Munniksmaa, A., Flachea, A., Verkuytenb, M., and Veenstra, R. (2012). Parental acceptance of children’s intimate ethnic outgroup relations: the role of culture, status, and family reputation. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 36, 575–585. doi: 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2011.12.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nesdale, D., and Flesser, D. (2001). Social identity and the development of children’s intergroup attitudes. Child Dev. 72, 506–517. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00293

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nosek, B., Greenwald, A. G., and Banaji, R. M. (2005). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: II. Method variables and construct validity. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 31, 166–180. doi: 10.1177/0146167204271418

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Bryan, M., Fishbein, H. D., and Ritchey, P. N. (2004). Intergenerational transmission of prejudice, sex role stereotyping and intolerance. Adolescence 39, 407–426.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Panno, A., Giacomantonio, M., Carrus, G., Maricchiolo, F., Pirchio, S., and Mannetti, L. (2017). Mindfulness, pro-environmental behavior, and belief in climate change: the mediating role of social dominance. Environ. Behav. doi: 10.1177/0013916517718887 [Epub ahead of print].

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Passiatore, Y., Pirchio, S., Carrus, G., Maricchiolo, F., Fiorilli, C., and Arcidiacono, F. (2017). Intercultural practices and inclusive education in Europe: can migration be a resource for individual and societal development? Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 1–16. doi: 10.1007/s10212-017-0360-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Patterson, M. M., and Bigler, R. S. (2006). Preschool children’s attention to environmental messages about groups: social categorization and the origins of intergroup bias. Child Dev. 77, 847–860. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00906.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pettigrew, T. F. (1991). Normative theory in intergroup relations: explaining both harmony and conflict. Psychol. Dev. Soc. 3, 3–16. doi: 10.1177/097133369100300102

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pettigrew, T. F., and Meertens, R. W. (1995). Subtle and blatant prejudice in Western Europe. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 25, 57–75. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2420250106

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piaget, J. (1946). La Formation du Symbole chez I’enfant [Symbol Formation of the Child]. Paris: Delachaux & Niestle.

Pinquart, M. (2016). Associations of parenting styles and dimensions with academic achievement in children and adolescents: a meta-analysis. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 28, 475–493. doi: 10.1007/s10648-015-9338-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinquart, M. (2017). Associations of parenting dimensions and styles with externalizing problems of children and adolescents: an updated meta-analysis. Dev. Psychol. 53, 873–932. doi: 10.1037/dev0000295

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pinquart, M., and Silbereisen, R. K. (2004). Transmission of values from adolescents to their parents. Adolescence 39, 83–100.

Google Scholar

Pirchio, S., Passiatore, Y., Carrus, G., and Arcidiacono, F. (2017a). Children’s interethnic relationships in multiethnic primary school: results of an inclusive language learning intervention on children with native and immigrant background in Italy. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 1–14. doi: 10.1007/s10212-017-0363-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pirchio, S., Passiatore, Y., Carrus, G., Maricchiolo, F., Taeschner, T., and Arcidiacono, F. (2017b). Teachers and parents involvement for a good school experience of native and immigrant children. J. Educ. Cult. Psychol. Stud. 15, 73–94. doi: 10.7358/ecps-2017-015-pirc

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pirchio, S., Taeschner, T., Colibaba, A. C., Gheorghiu, E., and Jursova Zacharová, Z. (2015). “Family involvement in second language learning: the Bilfam project,” in Early Years Second Language Education, eds S. Mourão and M. Lourenço (Oxon: Routledge-Taylor and Francis), 204–217. doi: 10.4324/9781315889948

CrossRef Full Text

Raabe, T., and Beelmann, A. (2011). Development of ethnic, racial, and national prejudice in childhood and adolescence: a multinational meta-analysis of age differences. Child Dev. 82, 1715–1737. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01668.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Radke-Yarrow, M., Trager, H., and Miller, J. (1952). The role of parents in the development of children’s ethnic attitudes. Child Dev. 23, 13–53. doi: 10.2307/1125889

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, C. C., Mandleco, B., Olsen, S. F., and Hart, C. H. (2001). “The parenting styles and dimensions questionnaire (PSQD),” in Handbook of Family Measurement Techniques, Vol. 3, eds B. F. Perlmutter, J. Touliatos, and G. W. Holden (Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage), 319–321.

Google Scholar

Rodriguez-Garcia, J. M., and Wagner, U. (2009). Learning to be prejudiced: a test of unidirectional and bidirectional models of parent-offspring socialization. Int. J. Intercult. Relat. 33, 516–523. doi: 10.1016/j.ijintrel.2009.08.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rutland, A. (1999). The development of national prejudice, in–group favouritism and self–stereotypes in British children. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 38, 55–70. doi: 10.1348/014466699164031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schnabel, K., Asendorpf, J. B., and Greenwald, A. G. (2007). “Using implicit association tests for the assessment of implicit personality self-concept,” in Handbook of Personality Theory and Testing, eds G. J. Boyle, G. Matthews, and H. Saklofske (London: Sage).

Google Scholar

Schneider, W., and Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing. I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychol. Rev. 84, 1–66. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.84.1.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sibley, C. G., and Duckitt, J. (2008). Personality and prejudice: a meta-analysis and theoretical review. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 12, 248–279. doi: 10.1177/1088868308319226

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinclair, S., Dunn, E., and Lowery, B. (2005a). The relationship between parental racial attitudes and children’s implicit prejudice. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 41, 283–289. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2004.06.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sinclair, S., Lowery, B., Hardin, C., and Colangelo, A. (2005b). Social tuning of automatic attitudes: the role of interpersonal orientation. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 89, 583–592. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.4.583

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevenson, H. W., Lee, S., Chen, C., Stigler, J. W., Hsu, C. C., and Kitamura, S. (1990). Contexts of achievement: a study of American, Chinese, and Japanese children. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Dev. 221, 1–2. doi: 10.2307/1166090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Strack, F., and Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 8, 220–247. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0803_1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thijs, J., and Verkuyten, M. (2014). School ethnic diversity and students’ interethnic relations. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 84, 1–21. doi: 10.1111/bjep.12032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thijs, J., Verkuyten, M., and Grundel, M. (2014). Ethnic classroom composition and peer victimization: the moderating role of classroom attitudes. J. Soc. Issues 70, 134–150. doi: 10.1111/josi.12051

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Ewijk, R., and Sleegers, P. (2010). Peer ethnicity and achievement: a meta-analysis into the compositional effect. Sch. Eff. Sch. Improv. 21, 237–265. doi: 10.1080/09243451003612671

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verhust, B., and Hatemi, P. K. (2013). Gene-environment interplay in twin models. Polit. Anal. 21, 368–389. doi: 10.1093/pan/mpt005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vertovec, S. (2007). Super-diversity and its implications. Ethn. Racial Stud. 30, 1024–1054. doi: 10.1080/01419870701599465

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vezzali, L., Giovannini, D., and Capozza, D. (2012). Social antecedents of children’s implicit prejudice: direct contact, extended contact, explicit and implicit teachers’ prejudice. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 9, 569–581. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2011.631298

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vittrup, B., and Holden, G. W. (2011). Exploring the impact of educational television and parent-child discussions on children’s racial attitudes. Anal. Soc. Issues Public Policy 11, 82–104. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-2415.2010.01223.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: children’s explicit and implicit prejudice, blatant and subtle prejudice, parenting styles, child-Implicit Association Test, prejudice intergenerational transmission

Citation: Pirchio S, Passiatore Y, Panno A, Maricchiolo F and Carrus G (2018) A Chip Off the Old Block: Parents’ Subtle Ethnic Prejudice Predicts Children’s Implicit Prejudice. Front. Psychol. 9:110. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00110

Received: 29 September 2017; Accepted: 23 January 2018;
Published: 09 February 2018.

Edited by:

Ann Dowker, University of Oxford, United Kingdom

Reviewed by:

Yuyan Luo, University of Missouri, United States
Wanze Xie, Harvard University, United States

Copyright © 2018 Pirchio, Passiatore, Panno, Maricchiolo and Carrus. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Giuseppe Carrus, giuseppe.carrus@uniroma3.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.