Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 06 March 2018
Sec. Cognitive Science
This article is part of the Research Topic Cognitive Aspects of Interactive Technology Use: From Computers to Smart Objects and Autonomous Agents View all 19 articles

Internet Search Alters Intra- and Inter-regional Synchronization in the Temporal Gyrus

\r\nXiaoyue LiuXiaoyue Liu1Xiao LinXiao Lin2Ming ZhengMing Zheng1Yanbo HuYanbo Hu3Yifan WangYifan Wang1Lingxiao WangLingxiao Wang1Xiaoxia DuXiaoxia Du4Guangheng Dong,*Guangheng Dong1,5*
  • 1Department of Psychology, Zhejiang Normal University, Jinhua, China
  • 2Center for Life Sciences, Peking University, Beijing, China
  • 3Department of Psychology, London Metropolitan University, London, United Kingdom
  • 4Department of Physics, Shanghai Key Laboratory of Magnetic Resonance, East China Normal University, Shanghai, China
  • 5Institute of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Zhejiang Normal University, Jinhua, China

Internet search changed the way we store and recall information and possibly altered our brain functions. Previous studies suggested that Internet search facilitates the information-acquisition process. However, this process may cause individuals to lose the ability to store and recollect specific contents. Despite the numerous behavioral studies conducted in this field, little is known about the neural mechanisms underlying Internet searches. The present study explores potential brain activity changes induced by Internet search. The whole paradigm includes three phases, namely, pre-resting state fMRI (rs-fMRI) scan, 6-day Internet search training, and post rs-fMRI scan. We detected the functional integrations induced by Internet search training by comparing post- with pre-scan. Regional homogeneity (ReHo) and functional connectivity (FC) were used to detect intra- and interregional synchronized activity in 42 university students. Compared with pre-scan, post-scan showed decreased ReHo in the temporal gyrus, the middle frontal gyrus, and the postcentral gyrus. Further seed-based FC analysis showed that the temporal gyrus exhibited decreased FC in the parahippocampal cortex and the temporal gyrus after training. Based on the features of current task and functions exhibited by these brain regions, results indicate that short-term Internet search training changed the brain regional activities involved in memory retrieval. In general, this study provides evidence that supports the idea that Internet search can affect our brain functions.

Introduction

Finding information through Internet search engines has become a common daily activity for people (Small et al., 2009). The widespread use of the Internet changed the way we find and store information. “Google effect” indicates that when people use the Internet as an external storage, they need to remember “where” it is instead of the information itself (Sparrow et al., 2011; Ward, 2013). These studies suggested that Internet search reduced the need for effort to process and remember information (Carr, 2010).

Recent studies explored the influence of Internet search on the brain. Nicholas suggested that the younger “Google generation” spends less time on individual questions and searches quicker, but the members of this generation show poorer working memory and are less confident about the answers they provided compared with the older generation (Nicholas, 2013). Small et al. (2009) showed that prior experience of using Internet search increased brain responsiveness in neural circuits involved in decision-making and complex reasoning in aged adults after short-term training. The brain activities of experienced Google users were broader than that of novices during searches (Small et al., 2009). A previous study found that people who obtained information through Internet-based search memory task would show lower accuracy when recalling information and are highly impulsive in novel trials (Dong and Potenza, 2015). However, 6 days of practicing Internet search improved their efficiency, but it reduced their dependency on their long-term memory (Dong and Potenza, 2016; Dong et al., 2017).

Brain and behavior are a dynamic system that influences each other and form the basis of brain plasticity (Mechelli et al., 2004). A large number of studies proved that learning induced brain plasticity (Kolb and Whishaw, 2003; Vartanian et al., 2013). In addition, various tasks generated differentiated functional response patterns through modulation of training (Koeneke et al., 2004; Kelly and Garavan, 2005; Erickson et al., 2007; Thomas et al., 2009). Previous studies revealed that 3 months of juggling training could lead to a transient bilateral expansion in gray matter in the mid-temporal area and in the left posterior intraparietal sulcus (Draganski et al., 2004). Five hours of meditation training can change the activity of default mode network and connectivity of the white matter (Brewer et al., 2011; Ding et al., 2015). The neural circuitry of the frontal pole, anterior temporal cortex, and anterior and posterior cingulate was activated in Internet-savvy subjects after 5 days of practice (Small et al., 2009).

Researchers recently realized that the effect of training/expertise-specific on brain functions may extend beyond task state to resting state. Resting state refers to the state when subjects relax, stationary, eyes closed, and avoid any systematic thinking (Mazoyer et al., 2001). Resting-state fMRI was also used to study intrinsic functional connectivity (FC) and has been widely used as a tool to assess large-scale networks in the human brain in both clinical and healthy populations (Wink et al., 2006; Tian et al., 2007; Buckner et al., 2013).

Regional homogeneity (ReHo) and FC are often used to evaluate brain activity synchronization in resting-state of healthy subjects and patients. ReHo is a rank-based non-parametric data-driven approach that reflects the temporal homogeneity of the regional BOLD signal (Sepulcre et al., 2010). ReHo measures the functional coherence of a given voxel with its nearest neighbors by calculating Kendall’s coefficient concordance (KCC) (Zang et al., 2004; Zuo et al., 2012). The test–retest reliability of ReHo has been found very high despite the physiological noise and preprocessing effect (Zuo et al., 2012; Zuo and Xing, 2014). FC measures the similarity of the time series of two relatively remote brain regions (Biswal et al., 1995). These two measures are often used together to detect local and remote brain activity synchronizations. Hence, combining the ReHo and FC analysis could provide additional information about brain activity synchronization induced by Internet search.

Numerous studies revealed that short-term training could alter the resting-state features of our brain. An existing study found resting-state coherence in the right medial motor cortex was increased by brief sensory motor intervention (Verrel et al., 2015). A study on acupuncturists found that training/expertise could modulate resting-state activity by increasing regional clustering strength (Dong et al., 2014). In a short-term simulated microgravity study, 72 h of -6° head down tilt (HDT) resulted in decreased ReHo in the right inferior frontal gyrus (Liao et al., 2013). Modulation of resting-state functional connectivity (rs-FC) in the parietal circuit was found after 4 weeks of daily training of an explicit sequence learning task (Ma et al., 2011). A study found that 4 weeks of working memory training increased rs-FC between the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and the precuneus, but decreased rs-FC between the mPFC and the right posterior parietal cortex (Takeuchi et al., 2013). These studies suggest that the resting-state features of the brain could be altered by short-term training.

Given these findings, we speculated that the brain activity in resting-state would be affected by search engines usage. In the present study, we first explore abnormal brain activity using ReHo analysis and investigate the FC between regions with altered ReHo and other brain regions. Previous studies found that people using Internet search as a tool for remembering new information showed lower brain activations in the middle temporal gyrus (Dong and Potenza, 2015) and regions along the ventral stream (Knutson et al., 2012). In addition, people using Internet search tools just need to remember where the information is stored instead of the information itself (Sparrow et al., 2011). Hence, we hypothesized that short-term training would make people to be better at remembering where information is stored (higher brain activities in regions along the dorsal stream) than the specific content (lower brain activities in regions along the ventral stream). We compared data on resting brain states between pre- and post-scan from 42 college student volunteers to examine the changes of brain activity induced by Internet search by measuring ReHo and FC.

Method and Procedure

Participants

The experiment complied with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki). The Human Investigations Committee of Zhejiang Normal University approved this study. This study was conducted in accordance with the approved guidelines. Forty-two university students were recruited through advertisements (22 males; 20 females; age: 21.4 ± 1.2 years). All participants provided written informed consent and underwent structured psychiatric interviews using the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) (Lecrubier et al., 1997) performed by an experienced psychiatrist. According to the MINI assessment, they were free of psychiatric disorders, including major depression, anxiety disorders, schizophrenia, and substance dependence disorders. All participants were medication-free and were instructed not to use any substances, including coffee, on the day of scanning. To obtain information regarding their Internet search behaviors, all subjects were assessed using an Internet-search-use questionnaire (Supplementary Material) (Dong and Potenza, 2015; Wang et al., 2016). Results showed that all participants were familiar with Internet search and used it regularly. We included Internet search experience as a covariate to exclude the impact on experimental results (Wang et al., 2016).

Experiment Procedure

The whole experiment consists of three steps, namely, pre-scan, 6 days of training, and post-scan.

Subjects were “trained” for at least 1 h per day for six consecutive days. In the experiment, subjects were asked to finish one of six search tasks randomly without repetition. Each search task consisted of 80 fill-in-the-blank items that required subjects to seek answers using an Internet search engine. Participants were informed that they will receive 20 Chinese yuan for their everyday participation. To elicit motivation in searching, the subjects were advised that the daily reward would be paid based on their real performance [20 accuracy rates (%)]. This reward premise was approved by the ethics committee. Participants who took the work seriously and finished with an accuracy rate of over 80% passed the training.

Image Acquisition

Functional MRI was performed on a 3.0 Tesla Siemens Trio scanner. The functional scan was acquired using gradient echo planar imaging sequence with the following parameters: [repeat time (TR) = 2 s, echo time (TE) = 30 ms, flip angle = 90°; interleaved sequence; 33 slice per volume; 3 mm thickness; field of view (FOV) = 220 mm × 220 mm2, matrix 64 cm × 64 cm, acquisition matrix = 64 × 64]. Each functional run included 210 imaging volumes for each participant and the scan lasted 7 min. All subjects were instructed to rest quietly in the scanner without falling asleep. Post-scan conforms to the same standard and parameter.

Data Pre-processing

Data pre-processing was conducted using Data Processing Assistant for Resting-State fMRI (DPARSFA1), a MATLAB toolbox for “pipeline” data analysis of resting-state fMRI (Yan and Zang, 2010; Song et al., 2011). DPARSFA is based on some functions in Statistical Parametric Mapping2 and Resting-State (REST3). The main preprocessing steps and parameters are listed as follows. The first 10 volumes of each functional time series were abandoned to avoid the instability of the initial fMRI signal, thereby leaving 200 volumes. Preprocessing included slice timing, head motion correction, and spatial normalization to a standard template. Participants with maximum translation that exceeds 2.5 mm or maximum rotation that exceeds 2.5° were excluded from further analysis. To reduce the effects of confounding factors, a regression of nuisance signals including cerebral spinal fluid, white matter, six motion vectors was performed. Following regression, detrending was performed and temporal filtering (0.01 to 0.08 Hz) was applied to the time series of each voxel to reduce low-frequency drift and high-frequency noise. Data for one subject were excluded according to the head-motion parameter (2.5 mm; 2.5°).

ReHo Analysis

Resting-state fMRI data without spatial smoothing were used for ReHo analysis with DPARSFA. Individual ReHo maps were generated by calculating the KCC of the time series of a given voxel with its nearest neighbors (26 voxels) in all directions on a voxel-wise basis. The calculated formula of ReHo is defined as follows:

W=(Ri)2n(R¯)2112K2(n3n),

where W is the KCC for a given voxel that ranged from 0 to 1. Ri is sum rank of the ith time point and n is the number of ranks; R¯ = [(n++1) K]/2 is the mean of the Ri; K is the voxel number among time series (27 voxels, one given voxel plus the number of its neighbors). To reduce the influence of individual variations in the KCC value, each standardized ReHo map was generated by dividing the raw ReHo map by the global mean ReHo. Spatial smoothing was conducted on the ReHo maps with a Gaussian kernel of 4 mm × 4 mm × 4mm full-width at half-maximum (Liao et al., 2013; Chen et al., 2015).

FC Analysis

A seed-based correlation approach was used for FC analysis. The seed was defined in the regions’ existing difference and analyzed by ReHo. We calculated the temporal correlation between these seed regions and every other voxel within the brain. These procedures were executed using DPARSFA software.

Post–Pre Analysis

To explore the differences between the pre- and post-training, a pair-sample t-test was performed on the normalized ReHo and FC maps with REST software. The result and statistical map were set at a combined threshold of p < 0.05 (AlphaSim corrected) and a minimum cluster size of 110 voxels.

Results

Regional Homogeneity (ReHo)

Compared with the pre-scan data, the neuroimaging data from post-scan were associated with the decreased ReHo values in the temporal gyrus, which include the superior temporal gyrus and the middle temporal gyrus, the middle frontal gyrus and the postcentral gyrus. Table 1 shows the detailed information for the brain regions with ReHo difference after training. Figure 1 shows the brain areas.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Brain areas showing Regional homogeneity (ReHo) difference between post- and pre-training.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Brain areas with increased and decreased ReHo in post-scan compared with pre-scan. Pair-sample t-test p < 0.05, AlphaSim corrected, voxel size = 3 × 3 × 3; T-score bars are shown on the right bottom. The voxels with hot colors represent increased ReHo after training, and cold colors indicate decreased ReHo after training.

Seed-Based FC of Altered ReHo Regions

Based on the ReHo results, the temporal gyrus and the middle frontal gyrus were selected as seed regions of interest for FC analysis. The temporal gyrus exhibited decreased FC with the parahippocampal cortex and the temporal gyrus after training whereas the temporal gyrus showed increased FC with the parietal gyrus. The middle frontal gyrus exhibited decreased FC with the middle temporal gyrus and the middle frontal gyrus after training (Table 2 and Figure 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Pre–post differences in seed-based FC in altered ReHo regions.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Brain areas with increased and decreased FC in post-scan compared with pre-scan. (A) The voxels with hot colors represent increased FC between the temporal gyrus and the parietal lobe after training, and cold colors indicate that the temporal gyrus shows decreased FC with the parahippocampal cortex and the temporal gyrus after training. (B) The middle frontal gyrus exhibited decreased FC with the middle temporal gyrus and the middle frontal gyrus after training. Pair-sample t-test p < 0.05, AlphaSim corrected; FC, functional connectivity.

Discussion

This study finds decreased ReHo in the temporal gyrus, the middle frontal gyrus and the postcentral after training. The temporal gyrus exhibited decreased FC with the parahippocampal cortex and the temporal gyrus after training, whereas the temporal gyrus showed increased FC with the parietal gyrus. The middle frontal gyrus exhibited decreased FC with the middle temporal gyrus and the middle frontal gyrus after training. This evidence of local functional homogeneity and interregional FC contributes to our understanding of the changed brain activity produced by Internet search.

The middle temporal gyrus located at the end of the ventral stream, which was described as the “what” stream, is involved in object identification and recognition (Mishkin and Ungerleider, 1982; Goodale and Milner, 1992). The Decreased ReHo and FC in the temporal gyrus supports our hypothesis that short-term training decreases brain activities in regions along the ventral stream. The parahippocampal cortex is a part of the limbic system, which plays an important role in memory encoding and retrieval (Ekstrom and Bookheimer, 2007). Evidence from patient studies suggested that the middle temporal gyrus is associated with long-term memory (Squire and Zolamorgan, 1991; Meulemans and Van der Linden, 2003). Functional neuroimaging study also found that the middle temporal gyrus is activated when individuals participate in memory encoding and retrieval processes of memory (Onoda et al., 2009). Previous studies found that Internet search decreases brain activation in the temporal gyrus (Dong and Potenza, 2016). Decreased ReHo and FC in the temporal gyrus might suggest that Internet search causes people to rely less on their long-term memory.

Results showed that short-term training increased FC between the temporal gyrus and the parietal lobe. The posterior parietal cortex is referred to as the dorsal stream or “where” stream, which is involved in visuospatial processing (Mishkin and Ungerleider, 1982). Therefore, increased FC might suggest that Internet search enhances the spatial information processing (Dong et al., 2017). The result is consistent with our hypothesis. The middle frontal gyrus plays an important role in selective attention, executive control, and working memory (Fuster, 2002). Decreased ReHo and FC in the middle frontal gyrus may suggest that individuals engage less in remembering something when faced with information that can be found in the Internet. According to Sparrow, people who used Internet search engines to access information show worse recall rates of information (Sparrow et al., 2011). Our results indicated that short-term Internet search training could alter the memory system, which further provided evidence for the hypothesis that Internet search enables people to be better at remembering where information is stored than the specific content.

As the Internet evolved into a useful tool in our daily life, people become susceptible to the unprecedented Internet search environment (Zickuhr and Madden, 2012; Loh and Kanai, 2016). Some researchers found that many people are increasingly relying on Internet search (Zickuhr and Madden, 2012; Yang et al., 2014; Wang Y. et al., 2017). People show irritability and depression when they cannot immediately find what they want (Block, 2008). These symptoms are similar with withdrawal symptoms of pathological Internet use (Davis, 2001). The executive function of Internet addiction subjects is impaired, whereas the inhibition control of Internet-using behaviors of those people is weakened (Wang L. et al., 2017). Our study found that the brain activities of Internet search users behaved differently. These people might engage in less effort in remembering something after short-term training.

Conclusion

This study found that the aberrant ReHo and FC were mainly distributed in the temporal gyrus and the middle frontal gyrus, which are responsible for long-term memory. Results provide evidence that support the idea that Internet search can affect our brain functions.

Limitations

Several limitations should be addressed. First, given financial and time constraints, we canceled the control group and pre–post resting-state data were not collected from the control group. We paid more attention to pre–post difference in one group of subjects. Second, we found it impossible to recruit university students that have no experience in Internet search. Thus, all subjects were familiar with Internet search. This sample group might affect training effect. Given the limitation of rs-fMRI studies, the explanations of the results rely on the brain functions of relevant brain regions, which lack direct support from behavioral or task data. Thus, the explanations are based on reasoning and the interpretation of the results is uncertain. However, the findings provide evidence that Internet search can affect our brain functions.

Author Contributions

XLiu and XLin analyzed the data and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. LW and YW contributed to experimental programming and data collection. XD contributed to fMRI data collection. GD and YH designed this research. GD and MZ revised and improved the manuscript. All authors contributed to and had approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by National Natural Science Foundation of China (30371023).

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00260/full#supplementary-material

Footnotes

  1. ^ www.restfmri.net
  2. ^ http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm
  3. ^ http://www.restfmri.net/forum/

References

Biswal, B., Yetkin, F. Z., Haughton, V. M., and Hyde, J. S. (1995). Functional connectivity in the motor cortex of resting human brain using echo-planar MRI. Magn. Reson. Med. 34, 537–541. doi: 10.1002/mrm.1910340409

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Block, J. J. (2008). Issues for DSM-V: internet addiction. Am. J. Psychiatry 165, 306–307. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07101556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brewer, J. A., Worhunsky, P. D., Gray, J. R., Tang, Y. Y., Weber, J., and Kober, H. (2011). Meditation experience is associated with differences in default mode network activity and connectivity. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 20254–20259. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1112029108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buckner, R. L., Krienen, F. M., and Yeo, B. T. T. (2013). Opportunities and limitations of intrinsic functional connectivity MRI. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 832–837. doi: 10.1038/nn.3423

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Carr, N. (2010). The Shallows: What the Internet Is Doing to Our Brains. New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Company.

Google Scholar

Chen, C., Xiu, D., Chen, C., Moyzis, R., Xia, M., He, Y., et al. (2015). Regional homogeneity of resting-state brain activity suppresses the effect of dopamine-related genes on sensory processing sensitivity. PLoS One 10:e0133143. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0133143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, R. A. (2001). A cognitive-behavioral model of pathological Internet use. Comput. Hum. Behav. 17, 187–195. doi: 10.1016/S0747-5632(00)00041-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ding, X., Tang, Y. Y., Cao, C., Deng, Y., Wang, Y., Xin, X., et al. (2015). Short-term meditation modulates brain activity of insight evoked with solution cue. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10, 43–49. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsu032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dong, G., Li, H., and Potenza, M. N. (2017). Short-term internet-search training is associated with increased fractional anisotropy in the superior longitudinal fasciculus in the parietal lobe. Front. Neurosci. 11:372. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dong, G., and Potenza, M. N. (2015). Behavioural and brain responses related to Internet search and memory. Eur. J. Neurosci. 42, 2546–2554. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dong, G., and Potenza, M. N. (2016). Short-term internet-search practicing modulates brain activity during recollection. Neuroscience 335, 82–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.08.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dong, M., Wei, Q., Zhao, L., Yang, X., Yuan, K., Zeng, F., et al. (2014). Expertise modulates local regional homogeneity of spontaneous brain activity in the resting brain: an fMRI study using the model of skilled acupuncturists. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 1074–1084. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Draganski, B., Gaser, C., Busch, V., Schuierer, G., Bogdahn, U., and May, A. (2004). Neuroplasticity: changes in grey matter induced by training. Nature 427, 311–312. doi: 10.1038/427311a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ekstrom, A. D., and Bookheimer, S. Y. (2007). Spatial and temporal episodic memory retrieval recruit dissociable functional networks in the human brain. Learn. Mem. 14, 645–654. doi: 10.1101/lm.575107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Erickson, K. I., Colcombe, S. J., Wadhwa, R., Bherer, L., Peterson, M. S., Scalf, P. E., et al. (2007). Training-induced functional activation changes in dual-task processing: an fMRI study. Cereb. Cortex 17, 192–204. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fuster, J. M. (2002). “Physiology of executive functions: the perception-action cycle,” in Principles of Frontal Lobe Function, eds D. T. Stuss and R. T. Knight (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 96–109. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195134971.003.0006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodale, M. A., and Milner, A. D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends Neurosci. 15, 20–25. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(92)90344-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, A. M. C., and Garavan, H. (2005). Human functional neuroimaging of brain changes associated with practice. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1089–1102. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Knutson, A. R., Hopkins, R. O., and Squire, L. R. (2012). Visual discrimination performance, memory, and medial temporal lobe function. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 109, 13106–13111. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1208876109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koeneke, S., Lutz, K., Wüstenberg, T., and Jäncke, L. (2004). Long-term training affects cerebellar processing in skilled keyboard players. Neuroreport 15, 1279–1282. doi: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000127463.10147.e7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kolb, B., and Whishaw, I. Q. (2003). Brain plasticity and behavior. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 49, 1–5. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.01210

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lecrubier, Y., Sheehan, D. V., Weiller, E., Amorim, P., Bonora, I., Sheehan, K. H., et al. (1997). The mini international neuropsychiatric interview (MINI). A short diagnostic structured interview: reliability and validity according to the CIDI. Eur. Psychiatry 12, 224–231. doi: 10.1016/S0924-9338(97)83296-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liao, Y., Miao, D., Huan, Y., Yin, H., Xi, Y., and Liu, X. (2013). Altered regional homogeneity with short-term simulated microgravity and its relationship with changed performance in mental transformation. PLoS One 8:e64931. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0064931

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loh, K. K., and Kanai, R. (2016). How has the internet reshaped human cognition? Neuroscientist 22, 506–520. doi: 10.1177/1073858415595005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ma, L., Narayana, S., Robin, D. A., Fox, P. T., and Xiong, J. (2011). Changes occur in resting state network of motor system during 4 weeks of motor skill learning. Neuroimage 58, 226–233. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mazoyer, B., Zago, L., Mellet, E., Bricogne, S., Etard, O., Houdé, O., et al. (2001). Cortical networks for working memory and executive functions sustain the conscious resting state in man. Brain Res. Bull. 54, 287–298. doi: 10.1016/S0361-9230(00)00437-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mechelli, A., Crinion, J. T., Noppeney, U., O’Doherty, J., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S., et al. (2004). Neurolinguistics: structural plasticity in the bilingual brain. Nature 431:757. doi: 10.1038/431757a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meulemans, T., and Van der Linden, M. (2003). Implicit learning of complex information in amnesia. Brain Cogn. 52, 250–257. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00081-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mishkin, M., and Ungerleider, L. G. (1982). Contribution of striate inputs to the visuospatial functions of parieto-preoccipital cortex in monkeys. Behav. Brain Res. 6, 57–77. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(82)90081-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicholas, D. (2013). Google Generation II: web behaviour experiments with the BBC. Aslib Proc. 63, 28–45. doi: 10.1108/00012531111103768

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Onoda, K., Okamoto, Y., and Yamawaki, S. (2009). Neural correlates of associative memory: the effects of negative emotion. Neurosci. Res. 64, 50–55. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2009.01.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sepulcre, J., Liu, H., Talukdar, T., Martincorena, I., Yeo, B. T., and Buckner, R. L. (2010). The organization of local and distant functional connectivity in the human brain. PLoS Comput. Biol. 6:e1000808. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000808

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Small, G. W., Moody, T. D., Siddarth, P., and Bookheimer, S. Y. (2009). Your brain on Google: patterns of cerebral activation during internet searching. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 17, 116–126. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181953a02

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, X. W., Dong, Z. Y., Long, X. Y., Li, S. F., Zuo, X. N., Zhu, C. Z., et al. (2011). REST: a toolkit for resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging data processing. PLoS One 6:e25031. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0025031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sparrow, B., Liu, J., and Wegner, D. M. (2011). Google effects on memory: cognitive consequences of having information at our fingertips. Science 333, 776–778. doi: 10.1126/science.1207745

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Squire, L. R., and Zolamorgan, S. (1991). The medial temporal lobe memory system. Science 253, 1380–1386. doi: 10.1126/science.1896849

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Rui, N., Hashizume, H., Sekiguchi, A., Kotozaki, Y., et al. (2013). Effects of working memory training on functional connectivity and cerebral blood flow during rest. Cortex 49, 2106–2125. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thomas, A. G., Marrett, S., Saad, Z. S., Ruff, D. A., Martin, A., and Bandettini, P. A. (2009). Functional but not structural changes associated with learning: an exploration of longitudinal Voxel-Based Morphometry (VBM). Neuroimage 48, 117–125. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.097

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tian, L., Jiang, T., Liu, Y., Yu, C., Wang, K., Zhou, Y., et al. (2007). The relationship within and between the extrinsic and intrinsic systems indicated by resting state correlational patterns of sensory cortices. Neuroimage 36, 684–690. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vartanian, O., Jobidon, M. E., Bouak, F., Nakashima, A., Smith, I., Lam, Q., et al. (2013). Working memory training is associated with lower prefrontal cortex activation in a divergent thinking task. Neuroscience 236, 186–194. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.12.060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verrel, J., Almagor, E., Schumann, F., Lindenberger, U., and Kühn, S. (2015). Changes in neural resting state activity in primary and higher-order motor areas induced by a short sensorimotor intervention based on the Feldenkrais method. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 9:232. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, L., Shen, H., Lei, Y., Zeng, L. L., Cao, F., Su, L., et al. (2017). Altered default mode, fronto-parietal and salience networks in adolescents with Internet addiction. Addict. Behav. 70, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2017.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Wu, L., Luo, L., Zhang, Y., and Dong, G. (2017). Short-term Internet search using makes people rely on search engines when facing unknown issues. PLoS One 12:e0176325. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0176325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, Y., Wu, L., Zhou, H., Xu, J., and Dong, G. (2016). Development and validation of a self-reported questionnaire for measuring internet search dependence. Front. Public Health 4:274. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2016.00274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ward, A. F. (2013). Supernormal: how the internet is changing our memories and our minds. Psychol. Inq. 24, 341–348. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2013.850148

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wink, A. M., Bernard, F., Salvador, R., Bullmore, E., and Suckling, J. (2006). Age and cholinergic effects on hemodynamics and functional coherence of human hippocampus. Neurobiol. Aging 27, 1395–1404. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2005.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yan, C., and Zang, Y. (2010). DPARSF: a MATLAB toolbox for ”pipeline” data analysis of resting-state fMRI. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 4:13. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2010.00013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yang, H., Chen, Y., Zheng, L., Xu, X., and Cao, X. (2014). Analysis of internet use behaviors among clinical medical students in China. BMC Med Educ. 14:67. doi: 10.1186/1472-6920-14-67

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zang, Y., Jiang, T., Lu, Y., He, Y., and Tian, L. (2004). Regional homogeneity approach to fMRI data analysis. Neuroimage 22, 394–400. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.12.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zickuhr, K., and Madden, M. (2012). Older Adults and Internet Use. Pew Internet & American Life Project. Available at: http://www.pewinternet.org/?/media/files/Reports/2012/PIP-Digital-differences-041312

Google Scholar

Zuo, X. N., and Xing, X. X. (2014). Test-retest reliabilities of resting-state FMRI measurements in human brain functional connectomics: a systems neuroscience perspective. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 45, 100–118. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.05.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zuo, X. N., Xu, T., Jiang, L., Yang, Z., Cao, X. Y., He, Y., et al. (2012). Toward reliable characterization of functional homogeneity in the human brain: preprocessing, scan duration, imaging resolution and computational space. Neuroimage 65, 374–386. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: internet-search, short-term training, regional homogeneity, functional connectivity, long-term memory

Citation: Liu X, Lin X, Zheng M, Hu Y, Wang Y, Wang L, Du X and Dong G (2018) Internet Search Alters Intra- and Inter-regional Synchronization in the Temporal Gyrus. Front. Psychol. 9:260. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00260

Received: 02 February 2017; Accepted: 16 February 2018;
Published: 06 March 2018.

Edited by:

Amon Rapp, Università degli Studi di Torino, Italy

Reviewed by:

Xi-Nian Zuo, Institute of Psychology (CAS), China
Maddalena Boccia, Sapienza Università di Roma, Italy
Andrew James Greenshaw, University of Alberta, Canada

Copyright © 2018 Liu, Lin, Zheng, Hu, Wang, Wang, Du and Dong. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Guangheng Dong, dongguangheng@zjnu.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.