Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 27 March 2018
Sec. Perception Science

Openness to Changing Religious Views Is Related to Radial Diffusivity in the Genu of the Corpus Callosum in an Initial Study of Healthy Young Adults

\r\nJiansong Xu&#x;Jiansong Xu1†Clayton H. McClintock&#x;Clayton H. McClintock2†Iris M. Balodis,Iris M. Balodis1,3Lisa Miller&#x;Lisa Miller2‡Marc N. Potenza,,,*&#x;Marc N. Potenza1,4,5,6*‡
  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Yale School of Medicine, Yale University, New Haven, CT, United States
  • 2Spirituality Mind Body Institute, Department of Counseling and Clinical Psychology, Teachers College, Columbia University, New York City, NY, United States
  • 3Peter Boris Centre for Addictions Research, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioural Neurosciences, Michael G. DeGroote School of Medicine, McMaster University, Hamilton, ON, Canada
  • 4Child Study Center, Yale School of Medicine, Yale University, New Haven, CT, United States
  • 5Department of Neurobiology, Yale School of Medicine, Yale University, New Haven, CT, United States
  • 6Connecticut Mental Health Center, New Haven, CT, United States

A quest orientation to religion is characterized by a search for answers to complex existential questions, a perception of religious doubt as positive, and an openness to change one’s religious views as one grows and changes. This orientation is inversely related to fundamentalism, authoritarianism, and prejudice and directly related to cognitive complexity, openness to experience, and prosociality. To date, the neural correlates of religious quest have not been investigated. This study assessed the relationships between measures linked to white-matter integrity and quest religious orientation among 24 healthy participants using diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) and the quest scale. A tract-based spatial statistical analysis whole-brain-corrected initially employing an accepted threshold (pTFCE < 0.05) and then applying a Bonferroni correction (pTFCE < 0.0042) identified a region of the genu of the corpus callosum as showing radial diffusivity measures being related to openness to change religious beliefs. When not employing a Bonferroni correction (pTFCE < 0.05), the openness-to-change subscale of the quest scale negatively correlated with radial diffusivity and mean diffusivity measures in extensive white-matter regions in both hemispheres that include the corpus callosum body, genu, and splenium, superior longitudinal fasciculus, forceps minor, external capsule, and inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus. No relationships were found with the other subscales. These findings suggest that a greater openness to change one’s religious views is associated with better white-matter integrity specifically in the genu of the corpus callosum and likely in a more extensive set of white-matter structures interconnecting widespread cortical and subcortical regions in the brain across hemispheres. They, furthermore, suggest structural similarities that may link this tendency to associated positive psychological traits, including creative cognition and post-traumatic growth.

Introduction

Proposed as a third fundamental approach to religion along with Gordon Allport’s intrinsic and extrinsic religiosity, quest orientation refers to a form of religiosity that embraces honesty, doubt, and openness in the face of existential questions and complex life circumstances (Batson and Schoenrade, 1991). A multidimensional construct, religious quest as captured by the quest scale includes the following domains: (1) a search for answers to complex existential questions, (2) a perception of religious doubt as positive, and (3) an openness to change one’s religious views as one grows and changes. Despite the centrality of this orientation to religion and its association with a number of positive psychological traits, which include prosociality, openness to experience, and post-traumatic growth (Calhoun et al., 2000; Batson et al., 2001; Simpson et al., 2010), little is known about the neural structures that support this type of religiosity. Here, using diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) measures, we studied how white-matter structures related to aspects of religious quest.

Religious quest is inversely related to aspects of close-mindedness that include fundamentalism and authoritarianism (Altemeyer and Hunsberger, 1992; Hunsberger, 1995; Duck and Hunsberger, 1999), which, in turn, may predict prejudice toward those outside one’s own identity group (Altemeyer and Hunsberger, 1992; Brandt and Van Tongeren, 2015). In contrast, studies suggest that quest orientation is related to less prejudice and greater post-conventional reasoning (Batson et al., 1993; Cottone et al., 2007). Moreover, individuals high in religious quest tend to display empathetic and prosocial behavior (Batson et al., 1993, 2001). Quest religious orientation is also associated with the personality trait of openness to experience (Simpson et al., 2010). Those with this orientation tend to have both greater cognitive complexity and preference for cognitive consistency (Batson and Raynor-Prince, 1983; Barrett et al., 2005). Additionally, in the aftermath of traumatic life events, the degree of post-traumatic growth has been found to directly correlate with the quest domain of openness to change religious views (Calhoun et al., 2000).

Though the neural correlates of quest religious tendencies have not been directly investigated, brain imaging studies on potentially similar features, particularly the construct of openness to experience, serve to provide some relevant context. As measured by the Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R; Costa and McCrae, 1992a,b), openness has been observed to positively correlate with activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) during cognitive tasks and in the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) during the resting state (DeYoung et al., 2005; Sutin et al., 2009). Openness is also associated with measures suggestive of better white-matter integrity in both hemispheres of the dlPFC, as well as the superior longitudinal fasciculus (SLF), corona radiata (CR), anterior cingulum, forceps minor, corpus callosum body, and regions adjacent to the middle frontal gyrus (MFG) and inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) (Xu and Potenza, 2012). These white-matter regions interconnect many cortical and subcortical regions in the brain. Additionally, the capacity for cognitive flexibility is associated with dlPFC activity and prefrontal measures related to white-matter integrity (Badre and Wagner, 2006; Nyhus and Barceló, 2009). Taken together, these findings suggest that connections bilaterally in the prefrontal cortex (PFC), the corpus callosum, and other cortical and subcortical brain regions may serve as a neural feature underlying religious quest.

Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) measures water diffusion in multiple directions (Assaf and Pasternak, 2008). It can be used to generate fractional anisotropy (FA) values by calculating the normalized standard deviation of axial (λ1) and radial diffusivities (λT) to index the degree to which the water diffusion deviates from isotropic diffusion in white matter, and mean diffusivity (MD) by averaging λ1 and λT to index the overall diffusivity (Assaf and Pasternak, 2008; Neil, 2008). FA correlates positively with the ratio of λ1 and λT, while MD correlates positively with the sum of λ1 and λT. Therefore, FA and MD are sensitive to different patterns of changes in λ1 and λT. For example, an increase in both λ1 and λT may significantly increase MD without significantly changing FA, and a decrease in λ1 along with an increase in λT may significantly decrease FA without significantly changing MD. Therefore, both FA and MD are often used as indexes of white-matter integrity, and a decreased FA and/or an increased MD value in the white matter is usually interpreted as reflecting poorer white-matter integrity (Alexander et al., 2007).

This study aimed to investigate in a preliminary fashion the relationships between measures related to white-matter integrity and aspects of quest orientation to religion. Based on findings of openness on the NEO being related to measures associated with better white-matter integrity and the possibility that openness on the NEO and related to the construct of openness to change on the quest scale, we hypothesized that scores on the openness-to-change subscale specifically would be positively correlated with measures related to better white-matter integrity in prefrontal areas, corpus callosum, and other regions that interconnect widespread cortical and subcortical regions across both hemispheres. We explored correlations with the two other subscales of the quest scale as we believe that this would represent a negative control condition that would provide discriminant validity for potential findings related to openness to change religious views.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Individuals were recruited by media ads and provided written informed consent to participate in the protocol that was approved by the Yale Human Investigations Committee. Participants were screened using the Structured Clinical Interview (SCID) for Axis I Psychiatric Disorders (First et al., 1995, 1997) and provided urine samples to assess recent use of cocaine, opioids, stimulants, marijuana, and benzodiazepines. Handedness was assessed by asking participants which hand they usually use, and left-handed individuals were excluded. Participants were excluded if any metabolites of these substances were positive in their urine samples. Other exclusionary criteria included pregnancy, current psychiatric diagnoses, or unstable medical conditions. We acquired both DTI and self-reported data relating to aspects of religious quest from 24 English-speaking young adults (9 females), with a mean age of 22.2 years [range: 18–27, standard deviation (SD) = 2.3]. Mean education was 15.0 years (SD = 1.7). Two individuals were of Hispanic ethnicity. Races included white (n = 15), black (n = 4), Asian (n = 3), and other (n = 2). One participant was married, while all others were single.

Quest Scale

The quest scale (Batson and Schoenrade, 1991) is a 12-item measure that assesses perceptions of religion as a quest, conceptualized by the authors as “openly facing complex existential questions… and resisting clear-cut, pat answers” (Batson and Schoenrade, 1991, p. 430). Each item is rated on a nine-point scale ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 9 (strongly agree). The scale has been found to have three domains, with scores for which each range from 4 to 36 and the domains relating to: (1) Readiness to Face Existential Questions without Reducing their Complexity (e.g., “I have been driven to ask religious questions out of a growing awareness of the tensions in my world and in my relation to my world”); (2) Self-Criticism and Perception of Religious Doubt as Positive (e.g., “For me, doubting is an important part of what it means to be religious”); and, (3) Openness to Change (e.g., “As I grow and change, I expect my religion also to grow and change”). Reliability for the quest scale is satisfactory, with alpha coefficients ranging from 0.75 to 0.81, and the mean total score in previous samples of young adults ranged from 59.4 to 60.5 (Batson and Schoenrade, 1991).

Scanning Procedures

DTI data were acquired with a 3.0T Siemens Trio scanner at the Yale Magnetic Resonance Research Center. Diffusion sensitizing gradients were applied along 32 directions using b values of 0 (b0 image) and 1000 s/mm2 (TR = 7400, TE = 115, matrix = 128 × 128, FOV = 256 mm × 256 mm). Forty contiguous slices parallel to the AC-PC line were acquired, and each slice was 3.0 mm thick (Xu et al., 2010). Two repetitions were acquired for averaging. A high-resolution T1 image was routinely acquired and examined by a neuroradiologist to identify any structural anomalies.

Image Processing

The procedure for DTI processing was described previously (Xu et al., 2010). FMRIB’s Diffusion Toolbox (FDT) and Tract-Based Spatial Statistics (Smith, 2004; Smith et al., 2006, 2007) from FMRIB’s Software Library (FSL v5.01; Smith et al., 2004; Woolrich et al., 2009) were used for image analyses. A set of mean images was created by aligning and averaging the two image sets from each subject and were used to construct the diffusion tensor using FDT. FDT typically generates maps of FA, λ1, MD, λ2, and λ3. The map of the perpendicular eigenvalue (λT) was generated by averaging the maps of λ2 and λ3.

TBSS was used to register the FA map of each subject into Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) template space. A mean FA map was created by averaging registered FA images from all subjects, and a mean FA skeleton was created by thinning the mean FA image (Smith, 2004; Smith et al., 2006, 2007). The aligned FA data of each participant were projected onto the mean skeleton by searching the area around the skeleton in the direction perpendicular to each tract, finding the highest local FA value, and assigning this value to the skeleton. The transformation matrices created for FA map registration were used to register λ1, λT, and MD maps. Skeletons for λ1, λT, and MD were generated using the same procedures for creating the FA skeleton.

The relationships between diffusion indices and quest subscale scores were assessed using whole-brain general linear regression models, created using GLM_setup in FSL. Whole-brain voxel-wise statistical testing was conducted using the “randomize” program with 5000 permutations. The “randomize” program uses permutation-based, non-parametric inferences to perform voxel-wise cross-subject statistics (Nichols and Holmes, 2002). Statistical thresholds for all image analyses were cluster p < 0.05 after family-wise-error (FWE) corrected for multiple comparisons using FSL’s threshold-free-cluster-enhancement (TFCE) algorithm. JHU ICBM-DTI-81 White-Matter Labels and JHU White-Matter Tractography Atlas provided by FSLVIEW (3.1) were used to identify the anatomical location of significant clusters in the brain (Mori et al., 2008, 2009). The function “fslmeants” from FSL was used to extract means of FA, λ1, λT, and MD from each significant cluster surviving correction for multiple comparisons during whole-brain analysis. These means were used to demonstrate the relationships between DTI parameters and quest subscale scores using scatter plots, and no further statistical analyses were performed on these measures. A correction for multiple corrections beyond those used in DTI studies for whole-brain analyses (e.g., a Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons involving three subscale measures and four DTI measures) was not employed.

Results

Figure 1 shows the mean scores for the three subscales of the quest scale, with the openness-to-change scores being particularly relevant to the study hypothesis and subsequent figures. When applying a Bonferroni correction for 12 comparisons (3 subscales by 4 DTI parameters; P < 0.0042), a genual region of the corpus callosum was identified in the correlation between openness-to-change scores and λT values (Figure 2). When not applying a Bonferroni correction, scores on openness to change negatively correlated with λT and MD values in extensive white-matter regions in both hemispheres (Figures 3, 4). Regions showing significant negative correlations between scores on openness to change and λT values included the corpus callosum body, genu, and splenium, cingulum, SLF, anterior and superior CR, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus, forceps minor, forceps major, external capsule, anterior limb of internal capsule, uncinate fasciculus, and sagittal stratum. Some, but not all, of these regions also showed significant negative correlations between scores on the openness-to-change subscale and MD. The implicated regions included the corpus callosum body, genu, and splenium, SLF, forceps minor, external capsule, and inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus. These significant correlations appeared driven by the λT values, because MD values represent the mean λT and λ1 values, and λ1 values did not show significant correlations with scores on the openness-to-change subscale. Scores on the openness-to-change subscale did not show significant correlations with FA values. Scores on the readiness-to-face-existential-questions and perception-of-religious-doubt-as-positive subscales did not show significant correlations with any DTI parameters.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. The bar graph shows the group mean scores on the three subscales of the quest scale. Error bars indicate standard errors (SEs) of the mean. “Ready” – Readiness to Face Existential Questions without Reducing their Complexity; “Doubt” – Self-Criticism and Perception of Religious Doubt as Positive; “Open” – Openness to Change Religious Beliefs.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Displayed are findings from correlations of the quest openness-to-change subscale scores and perpendicular diffusion (λT) values thresholded at p-value corrected using threshold-free cluster enhancement (pTFCE) at <0.0042. Significant voxels at a cluster threshold for whole-brain significance (k = 221) are shown in red. Green represents the center of white-matter tracts, as defined using the FMRIB58_FA_skeleton mask. L, left.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Correlations between scores on the openness-to-change measure and λT values. Red-yellow color on the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) T1 template indicates brain regions exhibiting significant correlations between scores of openness to change religious beliefs and λT values. For illustration, scatter-plots demonstrate correlations between scores of openness to change religious beliefs (Openness; y-axis) and mean λT values averaged across all clusters identified as significant in the whole-brain analysis. The number at the left bottom of each brain image indicates the z coordinate in the MNI space. L, left.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. Correlations between scores on the openness-to-change measure and MD values. Red-yellow color on the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) T1 template indicates brain regions exhibiting significant correlations between scores of openness to change religious beliefs and MD values. For illustration, scatter-plots demonstrate correlations between scores of openness to change religious beliefs (Openness; y-axis) and mean MD values averaged across all clusters identified as significant in the whole-brain analysis. Number at the left bottom of each brain image indicates the z coordinate in the MNI space. L, left.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this study represents the first to investigate relationships between measures related to white-matter integrity and quest religious orientation. Our a priori hypothesis was partially supported as λT values correlated with openness-to-change scores and not with readiness-to-face-existential-questions or perception-of-religious-doubt-as-positive subscale scores. This finding was observed when correcting for whole-brain voxel-based tract-based spatial statistical analyses and when further applying a Bonferroni correction for 12 analyses involving 4 DTI and 3 quest subscale measures. When not applying a Bonferroni correction, λT and MD values in extensive regions of white matter were related to openness-to-change scores and not with readiness-to-face-existential-questions or perception-of-religious-doubt-as-positive subscale scores. Specifically, greater openness-to-change scores were related inversely to λT bilaterally in the corpus callosum body, genu, and splenium, cingulum, SLF, anterior and superior CR, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus, forceps minor, forceps major, external capsule, anterior limb of internal capsule, uncinate fasciculus, and sagittal stratum. MD values in some but not all of these regions were also inversely to openness-to-change scores. Given that our a priori hypotheses were based on prior findings in a similar yet different area and concerns that stringent Bonferroni corrections may be ‘deleterious to sound statistical inference’ (Perneger, 1998, p. 1236), we will focus the discussion on non-Bonferroni-corrected findings while noting that the findings implicating the genu of the corpus callosum are the most statistically robust.

Despite the above-described correlations between the openness-to-change scores and λT and MD values, the former were not related to either λ1 or FA values. As diffusion-weighted MRI may be considered as indirect assessments of white-matter integrity, findings from these and other tensor-derived indices should be interpreted with caution. However, increases in both λ1 and FA have been linked to axonal loss, whereas increases in both MD and λT have been associated with demyelination (Song et al., 2002; Jones et al., 2013). The finding of significant negative correlations between λT and MD values and scores on openness to change suggests that better white-matter integrity in specific regions relates to a greater openness to change religious views. Different processes in the brain may influence λ1 and λT differently. Axonal degeneration has been associated with changes in λ1 value (Concha et al., 2006; Sidaros et al., 2008), while demyelination has been associated with increased λT values (Song et al., 2002). However, the current study investigated young adults who are unlikely to have experienced significant axonal degeneration or demyelination; thus, the findings are likely to reflect individual differences with respect to λT, possibly relating to degrees of myelination. This line of reasoning suggests that greater openness to change religious beliefs may be related to better myelination of multiple white-matter tracts, and this may relate to better communication among brain regions connected by these tracts (Au Duong et al., 2005; Greicius et al., 2009). Future studies using different approaches (e.g., investigating how functional connectivity at rest or during cognitive and emotional processing relates to openness to change religious beliefs) are warranted in order to examine how openness to change religious beliefs may relate to other aspects of brain structure and function.

The corpus callosum body and forceps minor interconnect middle and superior frontal regions and motor, sensory, and auditory cortices, and are critical to interhemispheric communication (de Lacoste et al., 1985; Schulte et al., 2005; Voineskos et al., 2010). The SLF interconnects the PFC and the parietal cortex, is important for communication between these brain regions and contributes importantly to executive control, cognitive process, and emotional modulation (Alexander et al., 1986; Clark et al., 2009; Sexton et al., 2009). The inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus connects inferior and lateral margins of the occipital lobe to the lateral PFC and contributes importantly to the processing of visual and spatial information (Catani et al., 2002; Schmahmann et al., 2008). The present finding of correlations between measures of openness to change religious views and measures suggestive of better white-matter integrity in the corpus callosum, SLF, forceps minor, external capsule, and inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus is consistent with the notion that a greater openness to change religious views is associated with more efficient integration of information among extensive brain regions with wide-ranging functions in both hemispheres.

Changing and adapting one’s religious views as one grows and changes may be considered a creative process, as it involves integrating multiple domains of knowledge and employing divergent forms of thinking. Thus, it is worthwhile to consider whether these findings converge with previous brain research on creativity. Takeuchi et al. (2010) found that creativity correlated with measures linked to white-matter integrity in widespread bilateral regions which overlap with the current findings in the frontal lobe and corpus callosum body, as well as white-matter regions adjacent to the striatum, temporo-parietal junction, and inferior parietal lobe. In addition, creativity has been associated with greater interhemispheric and intrahemispheric EEG (electroencephalogram) findings (Jaušovec, 2000), right prefrontal cortex activation (Howard-Jones et al., 2005), and corpus callosum volume (Moore et al., 2009).

The findings suggestive of greater interhemispheric communication observed in the current data are noteworthy, as research suggests that the left and right hemispheres each possess particular specializations and encode information differently (McGlone, 1984; Barrett et al., 1998; Kosslyn et al., 1998; Heilman et al., 2000). Thus, increased interconnection between the two hemispheres may facilitate a variety of cognitive strategies to be used when negotiating and reconciling one’s religious views with other sources of knowledge and experience. Similarly, extensive white-matter connections throughout the brain may allow for the integration of knowledge previously encoded in disparate and/or isolated neural networks (Heilman et al., 2003). The current results are consistent with the notion that creativity involves efficient integration of information in the brain and facility with diverse cognitive functions (Takeuchi et al., 2010). It should be noted, however, that a previous DTI investigation on personality and creativity found negative correlations between FA values and both creativity and openness to experience in inferior frontal white matter (Jung et al., 2010), suggesting that these two tendencies may be related to poorer white-matter integrity. Further investigation could uncover nuances in relationships between creativity and white-matter integrity.

Research has furthermore suggested that post-traumatic growth directly correlates with both creative cognitive capacities and openness to change religious views (Calhoun et al., 2000; Peterson et al., 2008). Traumatic life events often upend people’s sense of self and view of the world (Calhoun et al., 2000). In addition, greater levels of religiosity have been reported following traumatic and stressful life events (Park et al., 1996). Thus, the capacity to embrace new religious perspectives may contribute to the capacity for resilience and for experiencing positive change in the aftermath of difficult life circumstances. Consistent with this notion, previous studies have linked reduced white-matter integrity with post-traumatic stress symptoms and greater white-matter integrity with increased resilience (Frodl et al., 2012; Daniels et al., 2013; Sanjuan et al., 2013; van der Werff et al., 2017).

The current study has several limitations. It identified correlations between measures related to white-matter integrity and aspects of religious quest, but the cross-sectional nature limits insight into causality. The study also relied on a relatively small sample of young adults. Thus, future studies should examine larger samples to investigate the extent to which the findings replicate and more diverse groups, including but not limited to older individuals. Additionally, possible gender-related differences should be examined. Ideally, future studies should also acquire a larger number of diffusion directions. While the different scalar indices used in this study each summarize different properties of the diffusion tensor and it has been recommended to use multiple indices in studies (Jones et al., 2013; Soares et al., 2013), they are not wholly independent. However, a Bonferroni correction for multiple corrections identified a region of the genu of the corpus callosum as most robustly related to λT values. In non-Bonferroni-corrected findings thresholded at pTFCE < 0.05, the specificity of the findings to the subscale assessing openness to change religious views is in line with our a priori hypotheses based on our prior study of openness more broadly defined (Xu and Potenza, 2012). Additionally, the specificity of the relationship between openness to change religious views and both MD and λT and not λ1 or FA suggests a role for myelination in these relationships (Song et al., 2002; Jones et al., 2013), and this possibility should be examined in future studies. Furthermore, values of λ1 and λT are affected by crossing fibers, changes in fiber directions induced by local pathology (Wheeler-Kingshott and Cercignani, 2009; Jbabdi et al., 2010), and local neuropil packing density (Beaulieu, 2002). For these reasons, findings from the current study should be considered preliminary and reflective of measures that have been associated with white-matter integrity but may arise from multiple etiologies. Thus, interpretation is limited, and questions should be examined by other approaches.

Conclusion

The current findings suggest that greater openness to changing religious views is related to better white-matter integrity in multiple tracts, including those connecting extensive brain regions between the two hemispheres. The widespread structural connections suggest neural pathways by which the capacity to embrace new religious perspectives as one grows and changes may occur. Furthermore, the results suggest brain structural features that may link openness to changing religious views to other potentially associated positive psychological tendencies, including those related to creative cognition and post-traumatic growth. Future studies should investigate these currently speculative possibilities, especially in larger and more diverse samples.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of Yale Human Investigations Committee with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Yale Human Investigations Committee.

Author Contributions

LM and MP designed the study. IB acquired the data, and JX analyzed the data. CM and JX wrote the manuscript, while LM and MP revised the manuscript. All authors reviewed and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported in part by R01DA039136 from the National Institutes of Health, the Connecticut Mental Health Center, The National Center on Addiction and Substance Abuse, and a Centers of Excellence in Gambling Research Award from the National Center for Responsible Gaming.

Conflict of Interest Statement

No funding agencies had input into the content of this manuscript, and none of the authors have any relevant conflicts of interests. MP has: consulted for Ironwood, Lundbeck, Shire, INSYS, RiverMend Health, Opiant/Lightlake Therapeutics, and Jazz Pharmaceuticals; received research support (to Yale) from Pfizer, Mohegan Sun Casino, and the National Center for Responsible Gaming; participated in surveys, mailings or telephone consultations related to addictions, impulse-control disorders or other health topics; consulted for and/or advised to gambling and legal entities on issues related to impulse-control/addictive disorders; provided clinical care in a problem gambling services program; performed grant reviews for the National Institutes of Health and other agencies; edited journals and journal sections; given academic lectures in grand rounds, CME events, and other clinical or scientific venues; and generated books or book chapters for publishers of mental health texts.

The other authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful for the assistance that Sarah Yip, Chaim Kind, Candice Dwyer, and Trevor Friedman provided for this study.

Footnotes

  1. ^http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl/g

References

Alexander, A. L., Lee, J. E., Lazar, M., and Field, A. S. (2007). Diffusion tensor imaging of the brain. Neurotherapeutics 4, 316–329. doi: 10.1016/j.nurt.2007.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Alexander, G. E., DeLong, M. R., and Strick, P. L. (1986). Parallel organization of functionally segregated circuits linking basal ganglia and cortex. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 9, 357–381. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.09.030186.002041

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Altemeyer, B., and Hunsberger, B. (1992). Authoritarianism, religious fundamentalism, quest, and prejudice. Int. J. Psychol. Relig. 2, 113–133. doi: 10.1177/0886260517698281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Assaf, Y., and Pasternak, O. (2008). Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI)-based white matter mapping in brain research: a review. J. Mol. Neurosci. 34, 51–61. doi: 10.1007/s12031-007-0029-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Au Duong, M.-V., Audoin, B., Boulanouar, K., Ibarrola, D., Malikova, I., Confort-Gouny, S., et al. (2005). Altered functional connectivity related to white matter changes inside the working memory network at the very early stage of MS. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 25, 1245–1253. doi: 10.1038/sj.jcbfm.9600122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Badre, D., and Wagner, A. D. (2006). Computational and neurobiological mechanisms underlying cognitive flexibility. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 7186–7191. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0509550103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, A., Beversdorf, D., Crucian, G., and Heilman, K. (1998). Neglect after right hemisphere stroke a smaller floodlight for distributed attention. Neurology 51, 972–978. doi: 10.1212/WNL.51.4.972

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barrett, D. W., Patock-Peckham, J. A., Hutchinson, G. T., and Nagoshi, C. T. (2005). Cognitive motivation and religious orientation. Pers. Individ. Dif. 38, 461–474. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2004.05.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batson, C. D., Eidelman, S. H., Higley, S. L., and Russell, S. A. (2001). “And who is my neighbor?” II: quest religion as a source of universal compassion. J. Sci. Study Relig. 40, 39–50. doi: 10.1111/0021-8294.00036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batson, C. D., and Raynor-Prince, L. (1983). Religious orientation and complexity of thought about existential concerns. J. Sci. Study Relig. 22, 38–50. doi: 10.2307/1385590

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Batson, C. D., Schoenrade, P., and Ventis, W. L. (1993). Religion and the Individual: A Social-Psychological Perspective. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Batson, C. D., and Schoenrade, P. A. (1991). Measuring religion as quest: 2) Reliability concerns. J. Sci. Study Relig. 30, 430–447. doi: 10.2307/1387278

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beaulieu, C. (2002). The basis of anisotropic water diffusion in the nervous system - a technical review. NMR Biomed. 15, 435–455. doi: 10.1002/nbm.782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brandt, M. J., and Van Tongeren, D. R. (2015). People both high and low on religious fundamentalism are prejudiced toward dissimilar groups. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 112, 76–97. doi: 10.1037/pspp0000076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calhoun, L. G., Cann, A., Tedeschi, R. G., and McMillan, J. (2000). A correlational test of the relationship between posttraumatic growth, religion, and cognitive processing. J. Trauma. Stress 13, 521–527. doi: 10.1023/A:1007745627077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Catani, M., Howard, R. J., Pajevic, S., and Jones, D. K. (2002). Virtual in vivo interactive dissection of white matter fasciculi in the human brain. Neuroimage 17, 77–94. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, L., Chamberlain, S. R., and Sahakian, B. J. (2009). Neurocognitive mechanisms in depression: implications for treatment. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 32, 57–74. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.31.060407.125618

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Concha, L., Gross, D. W., Wheatley, B. M., and Beaulieu, C. (2006). Diffusion tensor imaging of time-dependent axonal and myelin degradation after corpus callosotomy in epilepsy patients. Neuroimage 32, 1090–1099. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.04.187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, P. T., and McCrae, R. R. (1992a). Four ways five factors are basic. Pers. Individ. Dif. 13, 653–665. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(92)90236-I

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, P. T., and McCrae, R. R. (1992b). Normal personality assessment in clinical practice: the NEO personality inventory. Psychol. Assess. 4, 5–13. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.4.1.5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cottone, J., Drucker, P., and Javier, R. A. (2007). Predictors of moral reasoning: components of executive functioning and aspects of religiosity. J. Sci. Study Relig. 46, 37–53. doi: 10.1111/j.1468-5906.2007.00339.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daniels, J. K., Lamke, J. P., Gaebler, M., Walter, H., and Scheel, M. (2013). White matter integrity and its relationship to PTSD and childhood trauma—A systematic review and meta-analysis. Depress. Anxiety 30, 207–216. doi: 10.1002/da.22044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Lacoste, M. C., Kirkpatrick, J. B., and Ross, E. D. (1985). Topography of the human corpus callosum. J. Neuropathol. Exp. Neurol. 44, 578–591. doi: 10.1097/00005072-198511000-00004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeYoung, C. G., Peterson, J. B., and Higgins, D. M. (2005). Sources of openness/intellect: cognitive and neuropsychological correlates of the fifth factor of personality. J. Pers. 73, 825–858. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00330.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Duck, R. J., and Hunsberger, B. (1999). Religious orientation and prejudice: the role of religious proscription, right-wing. Int. J. Psychol. Relig. 9, 157–179. doi: 10.1207/s15327582ijpr0903_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

First, M. B., Gibbon, M., Spitzer, R. L., and Benjamin, L. S. (1997). User’s Guide for the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Personality Disorders: SCID-II. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing.

Google Scholar

First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L., Gibbon, M., and Williams, J. B. (1995). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders. New York, NY: New York State Psychiatric Institute.

Google Scholar

Frodl, T., Carballedo, A., Fagan, A. J., Lisiecka, D., Ferguson, Y., and Meaney, J. F. (2012). Effects of early-life adversity on white matter diffusivity changes in patients at risk for major depression. J. Psychiatry Neurosci. 37, 37–45. doi: 10.1503/jpn.110028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greicius, M. D., Supekar, K., Menon, V., and Dougherty, R. F. (2009). Resting-state functional connectivity reflects structural connectivity in the default mode network. Cereb. Cortex 19, 72–78. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heilman, K. M., Blonder, L., Bowers, D., and Crucian, G. P. (2000). “Neurological disorders and emotional dysfunction,” in The Neuropsychology of Emotion, ed. J. C. Borod (New York, NY: Oxford University Press), 367–412.

Google Scholar

Heilman, K. M., Nadeau, S. E., and Beversdorf, D. O. (2003). Creative innovation: possible brain mechanisms. Neurocase 9, 369–379. doi: 10.1076/neur.9.5.369.16553

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howard-Jones, P. A., Blakemore, S.-J., Samuel, E. A., Summers, I. R., and Claxton, G. (2005). Semantic divergence and creative story generation: an fMRI investigation. Cogn. Brain Res. 25, 240–250. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunsberger, B. (1995). Religion and prejudice: the role of religious fundamentalism, quest, and right-wing authoritarianism. J. Soc. Issues 51, 113–129. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.1995.tb01326.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaušovec, N. (2000). Differences in cognitive processes between gifted, intelligent, creative, and average individuals while solving complex problems: an EEG study. Intelligence 28, 213–237. doi: 10.1016/S0160-2896(00)00037-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jbabdi, S., Behrens, T. E., and Smith, S. M. (2010). Crossing fibres in tract-based spatial statistics. Neuroimage 49, 249–256. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jones, D. K., Knosche, T. R., and Turner, R. (2013). White matter integrity, fiber count, and other fallacies: the do’s and don’ts of diffusion MRI. Neuroimage 73, 239–254. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.081

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jung, R. E., Grazioplene, R., Caprihan, A., Chavez, R. S., and Haier, R. J. (2010). White matter integrity, creativity, and psychopathology: disentangling constructs with diffusion tensor imaging. PLoS One 5:e9818. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0009818

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kosslyn, S. M., Thompson, W. L., Gitelman, D. R., and Alpert, N. M. (1998). Neural systems that encode categorical versus coordinate spatial relations: PET investigations. Psychobiology 26, 333–347.

Google Scholar

McGlone, J. (1984). Speech comprehension after unilateral injection of sodium amytal. Brain Lang. 22, 150–157. doi: 10.1016/0093-934X(84)90084-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, D. W., Bhadelia, R. A., Billings, R. L., Fulwiler, C., Heilman, K. M., Rood, K. M., et al. (2009). Hemispheric connectivity and the visual–spatial divergent-thinking component of creativity. Brain Cogn. 70, 267–272. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mori, S., Oishi, K., and Faria, A. V. (2009). White matter atlases based on diffusion tensor imaging. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 22, 362–369. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e32832d954b

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mori, S., Oishi, K., Jiang, H., Jiang, L., Li, X., Akhter, K., et al. (2008). Stereotaxic white matter atlas based on diffusion tensor imaging in an ICBM template. Neuroimage 40, 570–582. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neil, J. J. (2008). Diffusion imaging concepts for clinicians. J. Magn. Reson. Imaging 27, 1–7. doi: 10.1002/jmri.21087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nichols, T. E., and Holmes, A. P. (2002). Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: a primer with examples. Hum. Brain Mapp. 15, 1–25. doi: 10.1002/hbm.1058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nyhus, E., and Barceló, F. (2009). The Wisconsin card sorting test and the cognitive assessment of prefrontal executive functions: a critical update. Brain Cogn. 71, 437–451. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Park, C. L., Cohen, L. H., and Murch, R. L. (1996). Assessment and prediction of stress-related growth. J. Pers. 64, 71–105. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1996.tb00815.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Perneger, T. V. (1998). What’s wrong with Bonferroni adjustments? BMJ 316, 1236–1238. doi: 10.1136/bmj.316.7139.1236

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, C., Park, N., Pole, N., D’Andrea, W., and Seligman, M. E. (2008). Strengths of character and posttraumatic growth. J. Trauma. Stress 21, 214–217. doi: 10.1002/jts.20332

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sanjuan, P. M., Thoma, R., Claus, E. D., Mays, N., and Caprihan, A. (2013). Reduced white matter integrity in the cingulum and anterior corona radiata in posttraumatic stress disorder in male combat veterans: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Psychiatry Res. 214, 260–268. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmahmann, J. D., Smith, E. E., Eichler, F. S., and Filley, C. M. (2008). Cerebral white matter. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1142, 266–309. doi: 10.1196/annals.1444.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulte, T., Sullivan, E. V., Müller-Oehring, E., Adalsteinsson, E., and Pfefferbaum, A. (2005). Corpus callosal microstructural integrity influences interhemispheric processing: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Cereb. Cortex 15, 1384–1392. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sexton, C. E., Mackay, C. E., and Ebmeier, K. P. (2009). A systematic review of diffusion tensor imaging studies in affective disorders. Biol. Psychiatry 66, 814–823. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.05.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sidaros, A., Engberg, A. W., Sidaros, K., Liptrot, M. G., Herning, M., Petersen, P., et al. (2008). Diffusion tensor imaging during recovery from severe traumatic brain injury and relation to clinical outcome: a longitudinal study. Brain 131, 559–572. doi: 10.1093/brain/awm294

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Simpson, D. B., Newman, J. L., and Fuqua, D. R. (2010). Further evidence regarding the validity of the quest orientation. J. Psychol. Christ. 29, 334–343.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Smith, S., Jenkinson, M., Beckmann, C., Miller, K., and Woolrich, M. (2007). Meaningful design and contrast estimability in FMRI. Neuroimage 34, 127–136. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M. (2004). Overview of fMRI analysis. Br. J. Radiol. 77 Spec No 2, S167–S175. doi: 10.1259/bjr/33553595

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Johansen-Berg, H., Rueckert, D., Nichols, T. E., Mackay, C. E., et al. (2006). Tract-based spatial statistics: voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data. Neuroimage 31, 1487–1505. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.02.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Woolrich, M. W., Beckmann, C. F., Behrens, T. E., Johansen-Berg, H., et al. (2004). Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage 23(Suppl. 1), S208–S219. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Soares, J., Marques, P., Alves, V., and Sousa, N. (2013). A hitchhiker’s guide to diffusion tensor imaging. Front. Neurosci. 7:31. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2013.00031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Song, S.-K., Sun, S.-W., Ramsbottom, M. J., Chang, C., Russell, J., and Cross, A. H. (2002). Dysmyelination revealed through MRI as increased radial (but unchanged axial) diffusion of water. Neuroimage 17, 1429–1436. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2002.1267

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sutin, A. R., Beason-Held, L. L., Resnick, S. M., and Costa, P. T. (2009). Sex differences in resting-state neural correlates of openness to experience among older adults. Cereb. Cortex 19, 2797–2802. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhp066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Sassa, Y., Hashizume, H., Sekiguchi, A., Fukushima, A., et al. (2010). White matter structures associated with creativity: evidence from diffusion tensor imaging. Neuroimage 51, 11–18. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.02.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Werff, S. J., Elzinga, B. M., Smit, A. S., and van der Wee, N. J. (2017). Structural brain correlates of resilience to traumatic stress in Dutch police officers. Psychoneuroendocrinology 85, 172–178. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2017.08.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voineskos, A. N., Farzan, F., Barr, M. S., Lobaugh, N. J., Mulsant, B. H., Chen, R., et al. (2010). The role of the corpus callosum in transcranial magnetic stimulation induced interhemispheric signal propagation. Biol. Psychiatry 68, 825–831. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.06.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wheeler-Kingshott, C. A., and Cercignani, M. (2009). About “axial” and “radial” diffusivities. Magn. Reson. Med. 61, 1255–1260. doi: 10.1002/mrm.21965

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woolrich, M. W., Jbabdi, S., Patenaude, B., Chappell, M., Makni, S., Behrens, T., et al. (2009). Bayesian analysis of neuroimaging data in FSL. Neuroimage 45, S173–S186. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.10.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, J., DeVito, E. E., Worhunsky, P. D., Carroll, K. M., Rounsaville, B. J., and Potenza, M. N. (2010). White matter integrity is associated with treatment outcome measures in cocaine dependence. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 1541–1549. doi: 10.1038/npp.2010.25

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, J., and Potenza, M. N. (2012). White matter integrity and five-factor personality measures in healthy adults. Neuroimage 59, 800–807. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.07.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: neuroimaging, diffusion tensor imaging, white matter, religion, spirituality

Citation: Xu J, McClintock CH, Balodis IM, Miller L and Potenza MN (2018) Openness to Changing Religious Views Is Related to Radial Diffusivity in the Genu of the Corpus Callosum in an Initial Study of Healthy Young Adults. Front. Psychol. 9:330. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00330

Received: 09 October 2017; Accepted: 27 February 2018;
Published: 27 March 2018.

Edited by:

Arnaud Delorme, UMR5549 Centre de Recherche Cerveau et Cognition (CerCo), France

Reviewed by:

Chien-Te Wu, National Taiwan University, Taiwan
James Danckert, University of Waterloo, Canada

Copyright © 2018 Xu, McClintock, Balodis, Miller and Potenza. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Marc N. Potenza, marc.potenza@yale.edu

These authors have contributed equally to this work as first authors.

These authors have contributed equally to this work as last authors.

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.