Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 04 May 2018
Sec. Cognition
This article is part of the Research Topic Intra- and Inter-Individual Variability of Executive Functions: Determinant and Modulating Factors in Healthy and Pathological Conditions View all 40 articles

General Slowing and Education Mediate Task Switching Performance Across the Life-Span

  • 1Padova Neuroscience Center, Department of Neuroscience, University of Padova, Padova, Italy
  • 2IRCCS San Camillo Hospital Foundation, Venice, Italy

Objective: This study considered the potential role of both protective factors (cognitive reserve, CR) and adverse ones (general slowing) in modulating cognitive flexibility in the adult life-span.

Method: Ninety-eight individuals performed a task-switching (TS) paradigm in which we adopted a manipulation concerning the timing between the cue and the target. Working memory demands were minimized by using transparent cues. Additionally, indices of cognitive integrity, depression, processing speed and different CR dimensions were collected and used in linear models accounting for TS performance under the different time constraints.

Results: The main results showed similar mixing costs and higher switching costs in older adults, with an overall age-dependent effect of general slowing on these costs. The link between processing speed and TS performance was attenuated when participants had more time to prepare. Among the different CR indices, formal education only was associated with reduced switch costs under time pressure.

Discussion: Even though CR is often operationalized as a unitary construct, the present research confirms the benefits of using tools designed to distinguish between different CR dimensions. Furthermore, our results provide empirical support to the assumption that processing speed influence on executive performance depends on time constraints. Finally, it is suggested that whether age differences appear in terms of switch or mixing costs depends on working memory demands (which were low in our tasks with transparent cues).

Introduction

Many everyday life situations require cognitive flexibility, namely the capacity to adaptively select between multiple task-sets. Task-switching (TS) paradigms are a useful tool for representing such situations within an experimental context. A growing body of research has investigated the underlying cognitive mechanisms of TS, mostly referring to two types of performance costs: the switch and the mixing costs. Switch cost is measured as the difference between a repeat and a switch trial, both in terms of Response Times (RT) and errors, whereas the mixing cost arises as the difference between a repeat and a single-task trial.

The aging literature points to a deterioration in TS performance in the elderly, especially for the mixing cost (Kray and Lindenberger, 2000; Meiran et al., 2001; Adrover-Roig and Barceló, 2010; Lawo et al., 2012). Many accounts of the phenomenon have been put forward, some referring to mechanisms specific for TS (Kray and Lindenberger, 2000; Mayr, 2001) and others trying to integrate age-related difference across different functions, proposing a unitary account of such aging effects (e.g., Lindenberger and Baltes, 1994; Salthouse, 1996; Li and Lindenberger, 1999; Wasylyshyn et al., 2011): in these models, cognitive decline is mainly attributed to deterioration in an underlying domain, rather than considering single domains as separated entities. For this reason, they are referred to as “common cause models”.

One example of these models is the processing speed theory put forward by Salthouse (1996). This model proposes that age-related differences stem from a decline in processing speed, the pace at which simple operations are performed by the cognitive system. Support for this theory mainly comes from studies using hierarchical linear models, or similar ones, to investigate the amount of variance explained by measures of speed of processing, most often consisting of scores obtained in substitution tests; through this type of analyses, similar findings have been replicated for measures of working memory (WM; Salthouse, 1992), Trail Making Test (Salthouse et al., 2000), fluid intelligence (Salthouse et al., 1998), cognitive inhibition (Salthouse and Meinz, 1995; Verhaeghen and De Meersman, 1998; also see Puccioni and Vallesi, 2012a,b), recall, reasoning and spatial abilities (Salthouse, 1993): when the variance explained by speed of processing measures is removed from the models, the relationship of more specific cognitive abilities with age drops down. Importantly for the present study, also the relationship between TS and age was strongly attenuated when controlling for processing speed (Salthouse et al., 1998).

Even though an involvement of speed of processing in determining age-related differences in EFs seems therefore to be likely, it is to be noted that it might not be the only factor at stake (Keys and White, 2000; Verhaeghen et al., 2006; Bugg et al., 2007). Some studies on the Stroop effect, which is consistently found to be increased in older adults, still showed a significant effect of age after controlling for general slowing (Salthouse and Meinz, 1995; Bugg et al., 2007); similar results were also obtained with a card sorting test (Bugg et al., 2007), another task presumed to rely on inhibition and set shifting. Coming to TS, a meta-analysis by Verhaeghen and Cerella (2002) confirmed an effect of age beyond the general slowing, at least for TS global costs, that is, performance difference between blocks in which the participant has to switch between tasks and blocks with only single tasks. There are thus counterexamples in which correlations with processing speed, even though significant, are of moderate entity, and cannot fully account for age-related effects (cf., Salthouse et al., 1998).

The speed of processing theory proposes two mechanisms to be responsible for age-related slowing as a consequence of a lower speed of processing (Salthouse, 1996): (i) the limited time mechanism, according to which early operations may take too long and therefore leave no time for later, possibly more complex operations; (ii) the simultaneity mechanism, that is, the assumption that products of lower level computations should be concurrently available for later processing. Despite these assumptions appear to be crucial on a theoretical ground, empirical testing is still scarce. To better characterize the relationship of speed of processing with TS performance, we used a cuing paradigm in which the cue-to-target interval (CTI) was varied across blocks, giving the participants more or less time to reconfigure or inhibit a previously active task-set. If limited time and simultaneity mechanisms were to operate, we should expect looser correlations with a longer CTI: by having more time to prepare for the task at hand, the impact of speed of processing on the performance should indeed be reduced. On the other hand, if mixing and switch costs do not depend on limited time mechanisms, we should observe no differences in correlations with processing speed across CTI conditions. We therefore built linear models for the two CTI conditions, always including a measure of processing speed as one of the predictors, to test whether the impact of such and other variables is actually higher when temporal constraints are stricter.

Other variables included in the models were chosen to investigate the contribution of other factors that are likely to play a major role in age-related cognitive decline. In particular, the construct of cognitive reserve (CR) has gained popularity during the last two decades (Stern, 2002, 2009; Valenzuela and Sachdev, 2006), with a number of studies demonstrating that the capacity of the cognitive system to cope with aging is also a function of previous life experiences. It is fair to say that early works mainly linked cognitive reserve to educational level, as the construct of cognitive reserve itself stems from those studies demonstrating a negative relationship between education and the severity of Alzheimer’s Disease symptoms (Katzman et al., 1988; Stern et al., 1992; Katzman, 1993). Nonetheless, life experiences other than education are also often considered to mediate age-related decline in older adults, mainly concerning occupational attainment (Satz et al., 1993; Stern et al., 1994; Garibotto et al., 2008) and leisure time activities (e.g., Wilson et al., 2002).

Even though it is plausible that education, being a relatively early event in life, plays a major role on the neural and psychological development of an individual, testing different dimensions of the CR construct becomes crucial for a better development of clinical tools and theoretical understanding of the aging process. For this reason, we chose to use the Cognitive Reserve Index questionnaire (CRIq) as a measure of cognitive reserve (Nucci et al., 2012): this tool provides both an overall measure of cognitive reserve and subscales related to each of the three dimensions cited above (education, occupational attainment, leisure time). In this way, it was possible to test the role for each of the relevant activities in a standardized manner. Even though CR and speed of processing are most likely to be involved in age-related differences when it comes to TS, there are a number of other variables that have been described as important in the EF literature. As depression is fairly frequently found among older adults (e.g., Beekman et al., 1999; Noël et al., 2004), and has been associated with reduced cognitive control (e.g., Meiran et al., 2011; Vallesi et al., 2015), Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) (Beck et al., 1961) scores were used as one of the regressors in the analyses. Finally, mild cognitive impairment and dementia have been found to affect TS performance on both mixing and switch costs (e.g., Belleville et al., 2008; Schmitter-Edgecombe and Sanders, 2009). We therefore chose to administer the Montréal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA, Nasreddine et al., 2005), as a measure of general cognitive integrity, and used its scores as another regressor in our analyses. In summary, building such large models allowed us to test and control for a variety of factors that might selectively affect TS performance in aging, taking into consideration possible confounds with speed of processing effects. Moreover, it was also possible to assess an important aspect of the processing speed theory, which is not much tested in the TS literature, namely the assumption that speed of processing is particularly relevant under high temporal constraints (e.g., Wingfield et al., 1985): in the present case, this was done by varying CTI across blocks.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A total of 98 volunteers (45 female), ranging from 21 to 79 years of age, were recruited through Internet and flyers. The whole experiment took place in a single session of 1.5 h at the Department of Neuroscience in Padova. Before starting the experiment, participants read and signed a consent form specifying the aim of the study and possible risks; another form was then presented in which they declared whether they had previous or current history of neurological or psychiatric problems, whether they had taken drugs or alcohol in the last 24 h and whether they had normal or corrected-to-normal vision.

Test Description

All volunteers were administered 5 paper-and-pencil tests, and two computerized tasks: a TS paradigm and a Sustained Attention Reaction Time (SART) paradigm (not reported here)1. The sequence was fixed except that the computerized tasks were counterbalanced depending on the participant’s number (see Computerized Tasks). The order was: Edinburgh Handedness Inventory, Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA), computerized tasks (counterbalanced), Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) and the BDI. A final questionnaire, the CRIq was administered only to those participants above 30 years of age, since the test is based on life-long experiences and younger individuals tend to systematically show lower scores just due to their relatively young age. Descriptive statistics of the sample are reported in Table 1, which shows participants divided into 3 age groups (21–30, 31–60, >60 years old), as commonly done in the aging literature (e.g., Charles et al., 2003; Bialystok et al., 2005).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Average demographic data, scores and standard deviations (in parenthesis) for each experimental group.

Computerized Tasks

Two computerized tasks were implemented on E-prime and then administered to each participant: a TS paradigm and a SART paradigm. The order of administration was counterbalanced according to demographic variables such as age range (in decades), gender and years of education (in 3 ranges): those sharing these features were pseudo-randomly assigned to one of four possible sequences to obtain a roughly similar number of participants per counterbalancing order in each population layer. The TS paradigm was indeed divided into two separate blocks with short or long CTIs. Task switching blocks were always performed consecutively, and the order of presentation was counterbalanced; TS blocks could be either preceded or followed by SART thus leading to the four possible aforementioned sequences. For data collection, we used a Dell laptop computer (Intel core i5-3320M CPU; 4 GB of RAM) with Windows 7 OS. Stimuli were presented on a 15-inch color monitor with a white background. Participants performed every task with a distance of about 50 cm from the screen.

Task Switching Test

Each run of the TS paradigm was divided in 5 blocks: at the beginning participants had to perform two pure blocks consecutively, then the mixed block, finally two pure blocks again presented in reverse order with respect to the first two blocks in order to control for practice and fatigue effects. The two runs differed in CTI, that is, either 100 or 1200 ms (short and long CTI, respectively). The stimuli used were the letters A and E presented above or below a fixation cross.

In the pure blocks participants were asked to indicate either the identity of the letter at hand (verbal task) or its spatial location (spatial task): this was accomplished by pressing F or K on the keyboard, which were labeled as 1 or 2. In this way, the same keys were used for both the verbal and the spatial task, and the stimulus-response mapping was fixed across participants: F (labeled as 1) was to be pressed for A in the letter task, and when the letter appeared above the fixation cross in the spatial task; K (labeled as 2) had to be pressed for E, or when the letter was below the fixation cross depending on the task. Pure blocks began with instructions and 4 practice trials: at the end of practice participants were free to decide whether to start the test phase, or keep on with 4 extra practice trials. During practice, the experimenter monitored the responses and made sure that the participant understood the task to be performed. Moreover, during practice participants were also provided with a feedback indicating accuracy, and whether the response took too long: responses over 2000 ms were considered as non-responses. The practice phase with the pure block was repeated by 22 participants (on average, 4.5 extra trials each). The test phase consisted in 16 trials for each pure block: therefore, at each CTI, 32 trials were collected for each task, holding a total of 64 pure trials per CTI condition. In the mixed block a similar procedure was adopted: this time 16 practice trials had to be performed, always with a feedback; once again, participants were left free to decide whether to begin the test phase or to have another practice run: 24 subjects needed at least one extra practice (on average, 23.3 extra trials each).

The test consisted of 64 trials. Figure 1 provides a graphical example of the procedure. At the beginning of each trial a cue indicating the task at hand was presented on the screen for 100 or 1200 ms depending on the run: the cues were transparent rather than arbitrary with respect to the task to be performed, as they were the words SPAZIO (Italian word for “space”) or LETTERA (Italian for “letter”). After this interval, the target stimulus appeared on the screen with the cue still present in its position (to minimize memory demands) and the participant had 2 s to provide a response. After this deadline passed by, both the target and the cue disappeared for a variable inter-trial interval ranging between 500 and 1000 ms, and a new trial began. The procedures described in this study were approved by the Comitato Etico per la Sperimentazione “Azienda Ospedaliera di Padova”.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Experimental Paradigm. Each trial began with a fixation cross. Cues were then presented indicating the task at hand. The words “Spazio” and “Lettere” (Italian for “space” and “letters”, respectively) were used and stayed on the screen for the whole duration of the trial. The target appeared either after 100 or 1200 ms depending on the block (long and short Cue-Target-Interval, CTI, conditions), and stayed on the screen for 2000 ms. Finally, an Inter-trial-interval (ITI) varied randomly between 500 and 1000 ms and a new trial began.

Data Analysis

The first trial of each mixed block was not considered in the analysis to avoid confounds with starting costs (Altmann, 2007). Five participants were excluded for excessive inaccuracy: to determine a threshold, a binomial analysis was carried out spotting those individuals responding at chance level (<60.8% correct) on the task switching conditions. After this, error and post-error trials were removed (7.8% of the total) (Rabbitt, 1966; Falkenstein et al., 2000). At this point, the distribution of RT data was analyzed to ensure normality: the distributions for each condition showed a skewness above 1; for this reason we transformed the data with a logarithmic function (Ratcliff, 1993; Whelan, 2008). The natural logarithm was used for this transformation: after the transformation, all the distributions survived to Shapiro–Wilk test for sphericity. Finally, outlier trials were determined for each participant in each condition (i.e., pure block, repeat trials, switch trials for each CTI): a cutoff value of two standard deviations above/below the mean was used (3.8% of the total trials were excluded). Analyses were conducted using R, version 3.4.0. Mixing cost and switch cost were calculated for both RTs and accuracy as in the literature (Roger and Monsell, 1995): Mixing cost = Performance difference between Repeat switching block trials and Pure block trials; Switch cost = Performance difference between Switch and Repeat switching block trials.

A 2 × 3 mixed ANOVA was performed for each cost, with CTI as the within-subject factor and group (21–30, 31–60, >60 years old groups) as the between subjects factor. Separate ANOVAs were run for errors and RTs. For post hoc analyses, a Tukey’s HSD test was used. Further, multiple regressions were performed on raw data for mixing and switch cost at different CTIs. Predictors were: age, MoCA score, years of education, BDI, and SDMT score. The latter was intended as a proxy of speed of processing. The same procedure was then used to build linear models excluding the youngest group (30 years of age or below): in this case years of education were not included in the models and were replaced with sub-scales and total score of the CRIq.

Results

ANOVAs

Mixing Cost

Mean RTs and accuracy for each group and CTI are reported in Table 2 and Figure 2. Mixing cost in RTs decreased when more time to prepare was given to the participants, as evident from the main effect of CTI [F(1,90) = 261.57, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.744]. This effect was constant across groups as no interaction emerged between Group and CTI [F(2,90) = 0.27, p = 0.76, ηp2 = 0.008]. The absence of a main effect for Group [F(2,90) = 0.4, p = 0.67, ηp2 = 0.006] indicated that the mixing cost did not significantly change with age. As far as the error analysis was concerned, results were substantially similar to those reported for RTs: once again, there was a main effect of CTI [F(1,92) = 6.94, p = 0.01, ηp2 = 0.072] such that a longer time between cue and target was useful to correctly complete the task; no differences emerged between groups [F(2,92) = 0.785, p = 0.459, ηp2 = 0.017] and no interaction was found between group and CTI [F(2,92) = 1.831, p = 0.166, ηp2 = 0.039].

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Multiple linear regressions with mix cost at 100 and 1200 ms CTI as DVs.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Mix cost as a function of group and CTI. RTs were logarithmically transformed before calculating the mix cost.

Switch Cost

Mean RTs and accuracy for each group and CTI are reported in Table 3 and Figure 3. As for the mixing cost, a main effect of CTI [F(1,90) = 5.32, p = 0.02, ηp2 = 0.055] was found on the RT switch cost, consistent with the literature on task switching. Critically, however, also a main effect of group reached significance [F(2,90) = 7.24, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.139]; Tukey’s pairwise comparisons indicated that the oldest group showed higher switch costs than the youngest (p < 0.001) and than the middle-age groups (p = 0.036). No differences emerged between the latter two groups (p = 0.19). Finally, CTI x Group interaction was well far from significance [F(2,90) = 0.1, p = 0.9, ηp2 = 0.002], meaning that no group gained particular advantage from a higher preparation time in terms of switch costs. As far as errors were concerned, no significant effect was found either for CTI [F(1,92) = 0.355, p = 0.553, ηp2= 0.004] or Group [F(2,92) = 2.866, p = 0.062, ηp2 = 0.06].

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Multiple linear regressions with switch cost at 100 and 1200 ms CTI as DVs.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Switch cost as a function of group and CTI. RTs were logarithmically transformed before calculating the switch cost.

Linear Models

In order to test for a predictive role of speed of processing, cognitive reserve, mood, general cognitive integrity and age, 8 linear models were built: 4 including the whole sample, and 4 on a sub-population of older and middle-aged individuals only. Beside the sample, the first set of linear models differed from the latter in the measures of CR: as the CRIq was not administered to the youngest group, education was the only CR index in the whole-sample models. Results are summarized in the tables below (Tables 25). In general, nearly all models significantly predicted TS performance, but the amount of explained variance was consistently higher for the switch cost. This may be in line with the ANOVA’s results, indicating a main effect of group for switch cost only. As SDMT scores were negatively related with TS performance according to the models, it is conceivable that the older group, with lower scores on SDMT, showed higher switch costs. Also in line with ANOVAs is the drop of predictive power for both costs when the participants had more time to prepare: once again, as we observe that speed of processing is the most powerful predictor, then it is expected that models’ significance drops when time constraints are looser. Even though SDMT performance did not always correlate significantly with TS costs in all the conditions, it is to be noted that it did when correlated alone. The reason for this may be linked to shared variance between age and SDMT scores: correlation between these two variables was fairly high (r(91) = -0.53, p < 0.001, CI 95% [-0.66, -0.362]).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Multiple linear regressions with mix cost at 100 and 1200 ms CTI as DV.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 5. Multiple linear regressions with switch cost at 100 and 1200 ms CTI as DV.

Finally, education was found to negatively correlate with switch cost in the short CTI condition, whereas the mixing cost did not seem to be affected at all. Under all aspects cited above, models built excluding the youngest group were similar: the predictive power dropped at long CTIs, predictors accounted for more variance in the case of the switch cost, even though SDMT scores correlated significantly both with switch and mixing costs when considered alone. Furthermore, the subscale of CRIq for education, was still a valuable predictor for switch cost under high temporal constraints (i.e., short CTI). However, no other cognitive reserve variable had an impact on TS performance.

Discussion

The aim of this study was twofold: on the one hand, the influence on TS performance of a number of variables related to cognitive aging was tested. To the best of our knowledge, despite a vast literature on TS and aging, no previous study had considered at the same time the role of both potentially protective and adverse factors in this frame. On the other hand, some aspects of processing speed theory have often been underestimated. Despite the theorization of a processing stream in which products of low level operations should remain available for later processing in order to successfully perform cognitive tasks (Salthouse, 1996), few studies were concerned with testing the role of speed of processing under different temporal constraints. There is consistent evidence that cognitive aging may be partially attributable to a decline in speed of processing (Salthouse, 1996). However, high variability in the elderly population suggests that multiple factors, including life-experience, may play an important role in shaping decline of cognitive functions in aging (Stern, 2002, 2009; Vallesi, 2016).

In order to better characterize these aspects in the executive domain, we implemented linear models using classical TS indices as dependent variables. Speed of processing, as operationalized with SDMT performance, was found to be consistently related to both mixing and switch costs, with a tendency to account for more variance in the latter case. In general, significance of the models was higher when the participants had little time to prepare (i.e., shorter CTI of 100 ms). This effect was driven by a drop of predictive power of SDMT scores and age: when temporal constraints were lower (i.e., longer CTI), it is likely that the capacity to quickly carry out simple operations becomes less relevant. This is in line with Salthouse’s (1996) processing speed theory. His model posits that age differences arise because the ability to carry out simple operations deteriorates over time. Performance in cognitive tasks is consequently disrupted since products of low level-operations are not available when successive, higher order operations, have to be executed. These results replicate other studies in which temporal demands have been manipulated to test this possibility in other domains (Salthouse and Coon, 1993; Kliegl et al., 1994).

Furthermore, variations of SDMT weight at different CTIs implicate that the weight of the speed of processing factor varies as a function of cognitive demands (Salthouse, 1992). It is to be noted, however, that correlations between processing speed and TS measures were moderate, and do not fully account for between subjects variability, replicating previous results (Salthouse et al., 1998; Cepeda et al., 2001). On the other hand, age was a significant predictor for the switch cost, suggesting that age-related effects on the latter are partly independent from general slowing, consistent with the literature on EFs (Verhaeghen and Cerella, 2002; Verhaeghen et al., 2006; Bugg et al., 2007).

Summing up, it is important to recognize that, even though speed of processing measures account for a significant part of the variance in executive performance, age-related differences persist beyond this construct. On a more fine-grained analysis, the relevance of general slowing may interact with experimental manipulations such as time-constraints, an often underestimated aspect in aging studies.

The role of cognitive reserve (CR) was also tested in the linear models. Contrary to our expectations, most indices of CR did not mediate TS performance. The only exception was education, being it negatively related to switch cost in the shorter CTI condition. For the interpretation of this result it might be important to underline that education was also the only CR index to significantly correlate with SDMT performance [r(91) = 0.24, p = 0.02]. It is possible to speculate that a high education level protects against speed of processing decline, in line with the literature (Cabral et al., 2016; Ihle et al., 2016). This would also be consistent with biological accounts of processing speed linking this construct to white matter integrity (Kerchner et al., 2012; Haász et al., 2013; Kuznetsova et al., 2016); also the original conceptualization of brain reserve, and subsequently CR, began with studies observing the beneficial effects of high education on brain pathologies affecting white matter connectivity (Katzman et al., 1988; Katzman, 1993).

However, other CR measures did not show the same effect. It is to be noticed that results concerning the role of CR in age-related EFs decline are ambiguous. Contrary to other domains, a relevant number of studies failed to establish a link between life experiences and a better performance in EF tasks. For instance, Hindle et al. (2017) in a recent study reported no difference between high and low CR Parkinson Disease patients in tasks tapping EFs. However, a similar research of the same group, also on a PD sample, revealed that education promoted various cognitive functions including EFs (Hindle et al., 2014), a pattern compatible to our data. Also research on non-pathological aging population is inconsistent on this point. Cabral et al. (2016) found a relation between CR and EFs; however, CR was operationalized only with measures of education (years of education, school failure, school attendance), or strictly related to it (reading books, newspapers and number of languages spoken). When using a more complex CR questionnaire, correlations with EFs or related functions sometimes fail to emerge (Puccioni and Vallesi, 2012b; Léon et al., 2014; Arcara et al., 2017).

Coming to the results of the ANOVAs, the finding of comparable mixing cost across groups is at odds with previous literature, as most of the studies find an increase in older adults (Kray and Lindenberger, 2000; Meiran et al., 2001; Adrover-Roig and Barceló, 2010; Lawo et al., 2012). However, a number of authors also report results similar to our (DiGirolamo et al., 2001; Kray et al., 2002; West and Travers, 2008). Classically, the mixing cost has been proposed to reflect a differential load on WM between the pure and the mixed block (Los, 1996; Kray and Lindenberger, 2000): as two task representations have to be held in the second case, this might slow down performance. This view is consistent with the finding of increased mixing cost on non-cued paradigms, where the participant has to keep track of the sequence. Within this frame, an increased mixing cost in older adults is a direct consequence of WM decline in aging (Kray and Lindenberger, 2000); coherently, in non-cued paradigms older adults’ performance suffers the most (Kray et al., 2002).

However, several studies cast doubts on the role of WM in mixing cost. Parametrically varying the number of task-sets does not produce any larger cost (Kramer et al., 1999; Rubin and Meiran, 2005), nor increases age-related differences (Buchler et al., 2008). As for switch cost, mixing costs are present only with bivalent stimuli (Mayr, 2001; Rubin and Meiran, 2005), namely stimuli on which both tasks may be potentially executed (as it is the case in the present study); also, introducing bivalent stimuli in one task elicits cautious responding on the other tasks performed in the same block employing univalent stimuli (Woodward et al., 2003; Metzak et al., 2013), a phenomenon which is known as the bivalency effect and is especially present in younger adults (Rey-Mermet and Meier, 2015). Finally, Mayr (2001) demonstrated that without overlap between S-R representations, mixing costs are absent: when responses were mapped differently for each task, or stimuli could not cue the competing representation, a mixing cost failed to emerge. Taken together, these results suggest that the mixing cost arises because of task conflict (Rubin and Meiran, 2005), resulting from a bottom-up activation of the competing task set: if there is no bivalency, there is no competition; on the other hand, the mixing cost would arise only in situations in which the two task-set representations overlap to some degree. The proposal of a stimulus-driven nature of the mixing cost is in line with findings of reduced mixing costs after extensive practice (Kramer et al., 1999; Buchler et al., 2008), since practice would help to reduce ambiguity.

What follows from these data is that older adults would particularly benefit from experimental manipulations aimed at reducing task ambiguity. For instance, Hirsch et al. (2016) report of no age-differences in mixing cost when presenting univalent stimuli. It is also possible to lower task ambiguity through the employment of strategies aiding a correct representation retrieval. Inner speech disruption through articulatory suppression has been consistently found to increase mixing costs in adults (Emerson and Miyake, 2003; Miyake et al., 2004; Saeki and Saito, 2009). Moreover, several studies have found an interaction between age and articulatory suppression costs, or explicit verbalization benefits (Kray et al., 2008, 2010; Chevalier and Blaye, 2009): children’s and older adults’ performance benefited more from verbalization, and was more disrupted by articulatory suppression compared to young adults’ performance. Supposedly, inner speech should help to form a WM representation about the task to be performed (Baddeley, 1996), or solve task-set competition; consistent with this, cue transparency has been found to reduce mixing and switching costs (Mayr and Kliegl, 2000; Arbuthnott and Woodward, 2002; Miyake et al., 2004; Logan and Schneider, 2006; Grange and Houghton, 2009): also in this case, ambiguity on the task to be performed next is lowered. In the present experiment transparent cues for the implementation of the TS paradigm were chosen. Indeed, the use of Italian words for “space” and “letter” to cue the upcoming task was likely to considerably reduce the competition in task selection. In other kinds of cueing paradigms, not only the cue does not solve ambiguity between tasks, but it also may be particularly detrimental, since it embeds an extra processing demand, as demonstrated by the literature on restart costs (Allport and Wylie, 2000; Poljac et al., 2009), namely the cost associated with cued repetition trials vs. uncued trials. It is therefore likely that older adults, who may have more difficulties in solving the ambiguity or forming correct WM representations, may benefit when an informative cue is used.

Coming to the switch cost, it is still debated whether it arises from an ongoing inhibition of a previously active task-set (Allport et al., 1994; Wylie and Allport, 2000), or it is due to a process of task-set reconfiguration (Roger and Monsell, 1995; Mayr and Kliegl, 2000; Monsell, 2003) or a mixture of the two (e.g., Tarantino et al., 2016; see Vandierendonck et al., 2010 for a review). However, its increase in older individuals had already been found in a number of studies using the cueing paradigm (DiGirolamo et al., 2001; Mayr, 2001; Meiran et al., 2001; Kray et al., 2002; Reimers and Maylor, 2005; West and Travers, 2008; Adrover-Roig and Barceló, 2010). There is a vast literature on differential cognitive strategies across the life-span indicating that older adults use a more bottom-up stimulus-triggered (reactive) strategy than top–down (proactive) cognitive control, consistent with the idea of an executive deficit in aging (Braver et al., 2005; Paxton et al., 2006). The use of cognitive control itself, as classically conceptualized by Norman and Shallice (1986), is required when routines do not suffice for the correct execution of the task: the use of a transparent cue may not aid much performance on switch trials, since extra information is needed (e.g., retrieval of different S-R rules); on repeat trials, however, a clear indication of the task to be performed may solve interference with the other task, thus facilitating task selection as proposed above. In other words, while using a transparent cue may be useful for repeat trials, such a beneficial effect is usually not found when a reconfiguration is needed (i.e., in switch trials). In individuals with lower speed of processing, this might prove particularly difficult; as a consequence, a bottom–up strategy may be implemented more often in older compared to younger adults. Since the endogenous component of the switch cost, as conceptualized by Monsell (2003), would be larger in older adults, a higher switch cost is expected. In the present case, not only switch cost was higher across CTIs for the older adults, but differences emerged more neatly at the short CTI, where speed of processing has been found to be a more robust predictor of TS performance.

Conclusion

Even though the construct of cognitive reserve has gained popularity in the last decades, it is still controversial which specific life-experiences may actually protect from age-related decline in different cognitive functions (e.g., Puccioni and Vallesi, 2012b; Léon et al., 2014). The results of the present study showed that, among various proxies of cognitive reserve, only education is associated, under highly demanding time-constraints, with improved switch cost. On the other hand, common cause theories have been put forward to link cognitive decline in different functions to a general underlying cognitive domain. Among these, the speed of processing theory predicts poorer performance due to an inability to carry out simple operations at a high pace: if this was the case, we would expect speed of processing to play a more prominent role under high temporal constraints. Our results suggest that this is indeed the case: with short CTIs, our speed of processing measure predicted a significant portion of variance, while it did not with long CTIs. Finally, a pattern of increased switch cost and comparable mixing cost across the different age groups emerged in contrast with many the previous works. We suggest that the kind of cue employed (i.e., its degree of transparency) is the critical manipulation explaining the discrepancy of our results with previous studies. Despite it is conceivable that the choice of cue transparency may produce some consequences, these have not been systematically investigated so far, leaving a methodological issue in the TS literature that our study started to solve.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. All volunteers gave their written informed consent prior to participation. The protocol was approved by the Bioethical Committee of the Azienda Ospedaliera di Padova.

Author Contributions

AV designed the research project and designed the tasks. LM collected and analyzed the data. LM, CS, and AV wrote the manuscript.

Funding

This work was partially funded by the European Research Council under the European Union’s 7th Framework Program (FP7/2007-2013), Grant Agreement n. 313692 awarded to AV.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Giacomo Caiani for help in data collection. We also thank Fondazione Istituto di Ricerca Pediatrica Città della Speranza, Padova, for hosting some of our lab facilities.

Footnotes

  1. ^ This test was a modified version of that developed by Robertson et al. (1997). In the SART, participants were asked to respond to frequent ’go’ stimuli but maintain a readiness to withhold a response to rare and unpredictable no-go trials. In each trial, a single number appeared randomly in the center of the computer screen, and remained for 250 ms, before being substituted by a fixation cross that lasted for 900 ms. Participants had to press the “B” key on the keyboard if the stimulus presented was any number except the number “3”. The items were subdivided into a practice session of 18 trials (including 2 no-go trials) and a test session of 225 trials (including 25 no-go trials). The results of this task will be reported in future works as it falls outside the scope of the current study.

References

Adrover-Roig, D., and Barceló, F. (2010). Individual differences in aging and cognitive control modulate the neural indexes of context updating and maintenance during task switching. Cortex 46, 434–450. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2009.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allport, A., and Wylie, G. (2000). “Task switching, stimulus-response bindings, and negative priming,” in Proceedings of the 18th International Symposium on Attention and Performance: Control of Cognitive Processes XVIII, Windsor, ON, 35–70.

Google Scholar

Allport, D. A., Styles, E. A., and Hsieh, S. (1994). “Shifting attentional set: exploring the dynamic control of tasks,” in Attention and Performance XV: Conscious and Nonconscious Information Processing, eds C. Umiltá and M. Moscovitch (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press), 421–452.

Altmann, E. M. (2007). Comparing switch costs: alternating runs and explicit cuing. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 33, 475–483. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.33.3.475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arbuthnott, K. D., and Woodward, T. S. (2002). The influence of cue-task association and location on switch cost and alternating-switch cost. Can. J. Exp. Psychol. 56, 18–29. doi: 10.1037/h0087382

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Arcara, G., Mondini, S., Bisso, A., Palmer, K., Meneghello, F., and Semenza, C. (2017). The relationship between cognitive reserve and math abilities. Front. Aging Neurosci. 9:429. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00429

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baddeley, A. (1996). The fractionation of working memory. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93, 13468–13472. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.24.13468

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beck, A. T., Ward, C., and Mendelson, M. (1961). Beck depression inventory (BDI). Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 4, 561–571. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beekman, A. T., Copeland, J. R., and Prince, M. J. (1999). Review of community prevalence of depression in later life. Br. J. Psychiatry 174, 307–311. doi: 10.1192/bjp.174.4.307

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belleville, S., Bherer, L., Lepage, E., Chertkow, H., and Gauthier, S. (2008). Task switching capacities in persons with Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Neuropsychologia 46, 2225–2233. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bialystok, E., Martin, M. M., and Viswanathan, M. (2005). Bilingualism across the lifespan: the rise and fall of inhibitory control. Int. J. Biling. 9, 103–119. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00746.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Braver, T. S., Satpute, A. B., Keys, B. A., Racine, C. A., and Barch, D. M. (2005). Context processing and context maintenance in healthy aging and early-stage dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. Psychol. Aging 20, 33–46. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.20.1.33

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buchler, N. G., Hoyer, W. J., and Cerella, J. (2008). Rules and more rules: the effects of multiple tasks, extensive training, and aging on task-switching performance. Mem. Cogn. 36, 735–748. doi: 10.3758/MC.36.4.735

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bugg, J. M., DeLosh, E. L., Davalos, D. B., and Davis, H. P. (2007). Age differences in Stroop interference: contributions of general slowing and task-specific deficits. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 14, 155–167. doi: 10.1080/138255891007065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cabral, J. C., Veleda, G. W., Mazzoleni, M., Colares, E. P., Neiva-Silva, L., and Neves, V. T. (2016). Stress and cognitive reserve as independent factors of neuropsychological performance in healthy elderly. Ciên. Saúde Colet. 21, 3499–3508. doi: 10.1590/1413-812320152111.17452015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cepeda, N. J., Kramer, A. F., and Gonzalez de Sather, J. C. (2001). Changes in executive control across the life span: examination of task-switching performance. Dev. Psychol. 37, 715–730. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.37.5.715

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Charles, S. T., Mather, M., and Carstensen, L. L. (2003). Aging and emotional memory: the forgettable nature of negative images for older adults. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 132, 310–324. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.132.2.310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chevalier, N., and Blaye, A. (2009). Setting goals to switch between tasks: effect of cue transparency on children’s cognitive flexibility. Dev. Psychol. 45, 782–797. doi: 10.1037/a0015409

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DiGirolamo, G. J., Kramer, A. F., Barad, V., Cepeda, N. J., Weissman, D. H., and Milham, M. P. (2001). General and task-specific frontal lobe recruitment in older adults during executive processes: a fMRI investigation of task-switching. Neuroreport 12, 2065–2071. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200107030-00054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Emerson, M. J., and Miyake, A. (2003). The role of inner speech in task switching: a dual task investigation. J. Mem. Lang. 48, 148–168. doi: 10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00511-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Falkenstein, M., Hoormann, J., Christ, S., and Hohnsbein, J. (2000). ERP components on reaction errors and their functional significance: a tutorial. Biol. Psychol. 51, 87–107. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(99)00031-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garibotto, V., Borroni, B., Kalbe, E., Herholz, K., Salmon, E., Holtoff, V., et al. (2008). Education and occupation as proxies for reserve in aMCI converters and AD: FDG-PET evidence. Neurology 71, 1342–1349. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000327670.62378.c0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grange, J. A., and Houghton, G. (2009). Temporal cue–target overlap is not essential for backward inhibition in task switching. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 62, 2068–2079. doi: 10.1080/17470210802696096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Haász, J., Westlye, E. T., Fjær, S., Espeseth, T., Lundervold, A., and Lundervold, A. J. (2013). General fluid-type intelligence is related to indices of white matter structure in middle-aged and old adults. Neuroimage 83, 372–383. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hindle, J. V., Martin-Forbes, P. A., Martyr, A., Bastable, A. J. M., Pye, K. L., and Mueller Gathercole, V. C. (2017). The effects of lifelong cognitive lifestyle on executive function in older people with Parkinson’s disease. Int. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 32, e157–e165. doi: 10.1002/gps.4677

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hindle, J. V., Martyr, A., and Clare, L. (2014). Cognitive reserve in Parkinson’s disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 20, 1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2013.08.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hirsch, P., Schwarzkopp, T., Declerck, M., Reese, S., and Koch, I. (2016). Age-related differences in task switching and task preparation: exploring the role of task-set competition. Acta Psychol. 170, 66–73. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ihle, A., Oris, M., Fagot, D., Maggiori, C., and Kliegel, M. (2016). The association of educational attainment, cognitive level of job, and leisure activities during the course of adulthood with cognitive performance in old age: the role of openness to experience. Int. Psychogeriatr. 28, 733–740. doi: 10.1017/S1041610215001933

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katzman, R. (1993). Education and the prevalence of dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 43, 13–20. doi: 10.1212/WNL.43.1_Part_1.13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Katzman, R., Terry, R., DeTeresa, R., Brown, T., Davies, P., Fuld, P., et al. (1988). Clinical, pathological, and neurochemical changes in dementia: a subgroup with preserved mental status and numerous neocortical plaques. Ann. Neurol. 23, 138–144. doi: 10.1002/ana.410230206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kerchner, G. A., Racine, C. A., Hale, S., Wilheim, R., Laluz, V., Miller, B. L., et al. (2012). Cognitive processing speed in older adults: relationship with white matter integrity. PLoS One 7:e50425. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0050425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keys, B. A., and White, D. A. (2000). Exploring the relationship between age, executive abilities, and psychomotor speed. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 6, 76–82. doi: 10.1017/S1355617700611098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kliegl, R., Mayr, U., and Krampe, R. T. (1994). Time-accuracy functions for determining process and person differences: an application to cognitive aging. Cogn. Psychol. 26, 134–164. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1994.1005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kramer, A. F., Hahn, S., and Gopher, D. (1999). Task coordination and aging: explorations of executive control processes in the task switching paradigm. Acta Psychol. 101, 339–378. doi: 10.1016/S0001-6918(99)00011-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kray, J., Eber, J., and Karbach, J. (2008). Verbal self-instructions in task-switching: a compensatory tool for action-control deficits in childhood and old age? Dev. Sci. 11, 223–236. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00673.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kray, J., Li, K. Z., and Linderberger, U. (2002). Age-related changes in task-switching components: the role of task uncertainty. Brain Cogn. 49, 363–381. doi: 10.1006/brcg.2001.1505

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kray, J., and Lindenberger, U. (2000). Adult age differences in task switching. Psychol. Aging 15, 126–147. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.15.1.126

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kray, J., Lucenet, J., and Blaye, A. (2010). Can older adults enhance task-switching performance by verbal self-instructions? The influence of working-memory load and early learning. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2:147. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2010.00147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuznetsova, K. A., Maniega, S. M., Ritchie, S. J., Cox, S. R., Storkey, A. J., Starr, J. M., et al. (2016). Brain white matter structure and information processing speed in healthy older age. Brain Struct. Funct. 221, 3223–3235. doi: 10.1007/s00429-015-1097-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawo, V., Philipp, A. M., Schuch, S., and Koch, I. (2012). The role of task preparation and task inhibition in age-related task-switching deficits. Psychol. Aging 27, 1130–1137. doi: 10.1037/a0027455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Léon, I., García-García, J., and Roldan-Tapia, L. (2014). Estimating cognitive reserve in healthy adults using the cognitive reserve scale. PLoS One 9:e102632. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0102632

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, S. C., and Lindenberger, U. (1999). “Cross-level unification: a computational exploration of the link between deterioration of neurotransmitter systems and dedifferentiation of cognitive abilities in old age,” in Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory, eds L.-G. Nilsson and H. J. Markowitsch (Ashland, OH: Hogrefe & Huber Publishers), 103–146.

Google Scholar

Lindenberger, U., and Baltes, P. B. (1994). Sensory functioning and intelligence in old age: a strong connection. Psychol. Aging 9, 339–355. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.9.3.339

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Logan, G. D., and Schneider, D. W. (2006). Interpreting instructional cues in task switching procedures: the role of mediator retrieval. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 32, 347–363. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.32.3.347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Los, S. A. (1996). On the origin of mixing costs: exploring information processing in pure and mixed blocks of trials. Acta Psychol. 94, 145–188. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(95)00050-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayr, U. (2001). Age differences in the selection of mental sets: the role of inhibition, stimulus ambiguity, and response-set overlap. Psychol. Aging 16, 96–109. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.16.1.96

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mayr, U., and Kliegl, R. (2000). Task-set switching and long-term memory retrieval. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 26, 1124–1140. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.26.5.1124

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meiran, N., Diamond, G. M., Toder, D., and Nemets, B. (2011). Cognitive rigidity in unipolar depression and obsessive compulsive disorder: examination of task switching, Stroop, working memory updating and post-conflict adaptation. Psychiatry Res. 185, 149–156. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.04.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meiran, N., Gotler, A., and Perlman, A. (2001). Old age is associated with a pattern of relatively intact and relatively impaired task-set switching abilities. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 56, 88–102.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Metzak, P. D., Meier, B., Graf, P., and Woodward, T. S. (2013). More than a surprise: the bivalency effect in task switching. J. Cogn. Psychol. 25, 833–842. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2013.832196

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miyake, A., Emerson, M. J., Padilla, F., and Ahn, J. C. (2004). Inner speech as a retrieval aid for task goals: the effects of cue type and articulatory suppression in the random task cuing paradigm. Acta Psychol. 115, 123–142. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2003.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends Cogn. Sci. 7, 134–140. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00028-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nasreddine, Z. S., Phillips, N. A., Bedirian, V., Charbonneau, S., Whitehead, V., Collin, I., et al. (2005). The Montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 53, 695–699. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Noël, P. H., Williams, J. W., Unützer, J., Worchel, J., Lee, S., Cornell, J., et al. (2004). Depression and comorbid illness in elderly primary care patients: impact on multiple domains of health status and well-being. Ann. Fam. Med. 2, 555–562. doi: 10.1370/afm.143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Norman, D. A., and Shallice, T. (1986). “Attention to action: willed and automatic control of behaviour,” in Consciousness and Self-Regulation, Vol. 4, eds R. J. Davison, G. E. Schwartz, and D. Shapiro (New York, NY: Plenum), 1–18. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4757-0629-1_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nucci, M., Mapelli, D., and Mondini, S. (2012). Cognitive reserve index questionnaire (CRIq): a new instrument for measuring cognitive reserve. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 24, 218–226.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Paxton, J. L., Barch, D. M., Storandt, M., and Braver, T. S. (2006). Effects of environmental support and strategy training on older adults’ use of context. Psychol. Aging 21, 499–509. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.3.499

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poljac, E., Koch, I., and Bekkering, H. (2009). Dissociating restart cost and mixing cost in task switching. Psychol. Res. 73, 407–416. doi: 10.1007/s00426-008-0151-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Puccioni, O., and Vallesi, A. (2012a). Conflict resolution and adaptation in normal aging: the role of verbal intelligence and cognitive reserve. Psychol. Aging 27, 1018–1026. doi: 10.1037/a0029106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Puccioni, O., and Vallesi, A. (2012b). High cognitive reserve is associated with a reduced age-related deficit in spatial conflict resolution. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 6:327. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2012.00327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rabbitt, P. M. (1966). Errors and error correction in choice-response tasks. J. Exp. Psychol. 71, 264–272. doi: 10.1037/h0022853

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ratcliff, R. (1993). Methods of dealing with reaction time outliers. Psychol. Bull. 114, 510–532. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.114.3.510

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reimers, S., and Maylor, E. A. (2005). Task switching across the life span: effects of age on general and specific switch cost. Dev. Psychol. 4, 661–671. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.41.4.661

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

Rey-Mermet, A., and Meier, B. (2015). Age affects the adjustment of cognitive control after a conflict: evidence from the bivalency effect. Neuropsychol. Dev. Cogn. B Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 22, 72–94. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2014.889070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robertson, I. H., Manly, T., Andrade, J., Baddeley, B. T., and Yiend, J. (1997). ‘Oops!’: performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia 35, 747–758. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00015-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roger, R. D., and Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of a predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 124, 207–231. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.124.2.207

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubin, O., and Meiran, N. (2005). On the origins of the task mixing cost in the cuing task switching paradigm. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 31, 1477–1491. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.31.6.1477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saeki, E., and Saito, S. (2009). Verbal representation in task order control: an examination with transition and task cues in random task switching. Mem. Cogn. 37, 1040–1050. doi: 10.3758/MC.37.7.1040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A. (1992). Why do adult age differences increase with task complexity? Dev. Psychol. 28, 905–918. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.28.5.905

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A. (1993). Speed mediation of adult age differences in cognition. Dev. Psychol. 29, 722–738. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.29.4.722

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychol. Rev. 103, 403–428. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.103.3.403

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A., and Coon, V. E. (1993). Influence of task-specific processing speed on age differences in memory. J. Gerontol. Psychol. Sci. 48, 245–255. doi: 10.1093/geronj/48.5.P245

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A., Fristoe, N., McGuthry, K. E., and Hambrick, D. Z. (1998). Relation of task switching to speed, age, and fluid intelligence. Psychol. Aging 13, 445–461. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.13.3.445

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A., and Meinz, E. J. (1995). Aging, inhibition, working memory, and speed. J. Gerontol. Ser. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 50, 297–306.

Google Scholar

Salthouse, T. A., Toth, J., Daniels, K., Parks, C., Pak, R., Wolbrette, M., et al. (2000). Effects of aging on efficiency of task switching in a variant of the trail making test. Neuropsychology 14, 102–111. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.14.1.102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Satz, P., Morgenstern, H., Miller, E. N., Seines, O. A., Mcarthur, J. C., Cohen, B. A., et al. (1993). Low education as a possible risk factor for cognitive abnormalities in HIV-1: findings from the Multicenter AIDS Cohort Study (MACS). J. Acquir. Immune Defic. Syndr. 6, 503–511. doi: 10.1097/00126334-199305000-00011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmitter-Edgecombe, M., and Sanders, C. (2009). Task switching in mild cognitive impairment: switch and nonswitch costs. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 15, 103–111. doi: 10.1017/S1355617708090140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, Y. (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 8, 448–460. doi: 10.1017/S1355617702813248

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, Y. (2009). Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia 47, 2015–2028. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, Y., Alexander, G. E., Prohovnik, I., and Mayeux, R. (1992). Inverse relationship between education and parieto temporal perfusion deficit in Alzheimer’s disease. Ann. Neurol. 32, 371–375. doi: 10.1002/ana.410320311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stern, Y., Gurland, B., Tatemichi, T. K., Tang, M. X., Wilder, D., and Mayeux, R. (1994). Influence of education and occupation on the incidence of Alzheimer’s disease. JAMA 271, 1004–1010. doi: 10.1001/jama.1994.03510370056032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tarantino, V., Mazzonetto, I., and Vallesi, A. (2016). Electrophysiological correlates of the cognitive control processes underpinning mixing and switching costs. Brain Res. 1646, 160–173. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.05.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valenzuela, M. J., and Sachdev, P. (2006). Brain reserve and dementia: a systematic review. Psychol. Med. 36, 441–454. doi: 10.1017/S0033291705006264

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallesi, A. (2016). Dual-task costs in aging are predicted by formal education. Aging Clin. Exp. Res. 28, 959–964. doi: 10.1007/s40520-015-0385-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vallesi, A., Canalaz, F., Balestrieri, M., and Brambilla, P. (2015). Modulating speed-accuracy strategies in major depression. J. Psychiatr. Res. 60, 103–108. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.09.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vandierendonck, A., Liefooghe, B., and Verbruggen, F. (2010). Task switching: interplay of reconfiguration and interference control. Psychol. Bull. 136, 601–626. doi: 10.1037/a0019791

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verhaeghen, P., and Cerella, J. (2002). Aging, executive control, and attention: a review of meta-analyses. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 26, 849–857. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(02)00071-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verhaeghen, P., Cerella, J., and Basak, C. (2006). Aging, task complexity, and efficiency modes: the influence of working memory involvement on age differences in response times for verbal and visuospatial tasks. Aging Neuropsychol. Cogn. 13, 254–280. doi: 10.1080/138255890969267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Verhaeghen, P., and De Meersman, L. (1998). Aging and the Stroop effect: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Aging 13, 120–126. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.13.1.120

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wasylyshyn, C., Verhaeghen, P., and Sliwinski, M. J. (2011). Aging and task switching: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Aging 26, 15–20. doi: 10.1037/a0020912

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

West, R., and Travers, S. (2008). Differential effects of aging on processes underlying task switching. Brain Cogn. 68, 67–80. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2008.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whelan, R. (2008). Effective analysis of reaction time data. Psychol. Rec. 58, 475–482. doi: 10.1007/BF03395630

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, R. S., Bennett, D. A., Bienias, J. L., Aggarwal, N. T., Mendes De Leon, C. F., Morris, M. C., et al. (2002). Cognitive activity and incident AD in a population-based sample of older persons. Neurology 59, 1910–1914. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000036905.59156.A1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wingfield, A., Poon, L. W., Lombardi, L., and Lowe, D. (1985). Speed of processing in normal aging: effects of speech rate, linguistic structure, and processing time. J. Gerontol. 40, 579–585. doi: 10.1093/geronj/40.5.579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woodward, T. S., Meier, B., Tipper, C., and Graf, P. (2003). Bivalency is costly: bivalent stimuli elicit cautious responding. Exp. Psychol. 50, 233–238. doi: 10.1026//1618-3169.50.4.233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wylie, G., and Allport, A. (2000). Task switching and the measurement of “switch costs”. Psychol. Res. 63, 212–233. doi: 10.1007/s004269900003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: task-switching, speed of processing, transparent cue, cognitive aging, cognitive reserve

Citation: Moretti L, Semenza C and Vallesi A (2018) General Slowing and Education Mediate Task Switching Performance Across the Life-Span. Front. Psychol. 9:630. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00630

Received: 29 January 2018; Accepted: 16 April 2018;
Published: 04 May 2018.

Edited by:

Roberta Sellaro, Leiden University, Netherlands

Reviewed by:

Tilo Strobach, Medical School Hamburg, Germany
Francisco Barceló, Universitat de les Illes Balears, Spain

Copyright © 2018 Moretti, Semenza and Vallesi. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Antonino Vallesi, antonino.vallesi@unipd.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.