Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 06 July 2017
Sec. Molecular Psychiatry

A Gene-Based Analysis of Acoustic Startle Latency

\r\nAlicia K. Smith,Alicia K. Smith1,2Tanja JovanovicTanja Jovanovic2Varun KilaruVarun Kilaru2Adriana LoriAdriana Lori2Lauren GenslerLauren Gensler2Samuel S. LeeSamuel S. Lee3Seth Davin Norrholm,Seth Davin Norrholm2,4Nicholas MassaNicholas Massa4Bruce Cuthbert,Bruce Cuthbert2,4Bekh Bradley,Bekh Bradley2,4Kerry J. ResslerKerry J. Ressler5Erica Duncan,*\r\nErica Duncan2,4*
  • 1Department of Gynecology and Obstetrics, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, United States
  • 2Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, United States
  • 3Department of Emergency Medicine, University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, Dallas, TX, United States
  • 4Mental Health Service Line, Atlanta Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Decatur, GA, United States
  • 5Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States

Latency of the acoustic startle response is the time required from the presentation of startling auditory stimulus until the startle response is elicited and provides an index of neural processing speed. Latency is prolonged in subjects with schizophrenia compared to controls in some but not all studies and is 68–90% heritable in baseline startle trials. In order to determine the genetic association with latency as a potential inroad into genetically based vulnerability to psychosis, we conducted a gene-based study of latency followed by an independent replication study of significant gene findings with a single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP)-based analysis of schizophrenia and control subjects. 313 subjects from an urban population of low socioeconomic status with mixed psychiatric diagnoses were included in the gene-based study. Startle testing was conducted using a Biopac M150 system according to our published methods. Genotyping was performed with the Omni-Quad 1M or the Omni Express BeadChip. The replication study was conducted on 154 schizophrenia subjects and 123 psychiatric controls. Genetic analyses were conducted with Illumina Human Omni1-Quad and OmniExpress BeadChips. Twenty-nine SNPs were selected from four genes that were significant in the gene-based analysis and also associated with startle and/or schizophrenia in the literature. Linear regressions on latency were conducted, controlling for age, race, and diagnosis as a dichotomous variable. In the gene-based study, 2,870 genes demonstrated the evidence of association after correction for multiple comparisons (false discovery rate < 0.05). Pathway analysis of these genes revealed enrichment for relevant biological processes including neural transmission (p = 0.0029), synaptic transmission (p = 0.0032), and neuronal development (p = 0.024). The subsequent SNP-based replication analysis revealed a strong association of onset latency with the SNP rs901561 on the neuregulin gene (NRG1) in an additive model (beta = 0.21, p = 0.001), indicating that subjects with the AA and AG genotypes had slower mean latency than subjects with GG genotype. In conclusion, startle latency, a highly heritable measure that is slowed in schizophrenia, may be a useful biological probe for genetic contributions to psychotic disorders. Our analyses in two independent populations point to a significant prediction of startle latency by genetic variation in NRG1.

Introduction

Schizophrenia is a psychiatric disorder associated with significant morbidity, mortality, and global burden of disease with a prevalence estimated at 1% of the general population (1, 2). Although the cause is multifactorial, the strongest risk factor is family history of the disease, and high heritability up to 80% points to a major role for genetic variation in the etiology of schizophrenia (3). However, schizophrenia does not appear to be inherited in a Mendelian manner and genetic studies of schizophrenia have been complicated by both phenotypic and genetic heterogeneities. Recent genome-wide association studies (GWAS) have made promising steps in identifying independent genomic loci that infer polygenic lifetime risk and heritability to schizophrenia (46), but challenges remain in fully deciphering the genetic component of the disease.

One emerging strategy to approach the genetics of schizophrenia is the use of endophenotypes, measurable traits discoverable by a biologic test. To qualify as an endophenotype, the trait must be associated with the disease and be present whether or not the disease is active. The trait must also be heritable, co-segregate with the disease within families, and be found in non-affected family members at a higher rate than the general population (7). The advantage of using endophenotypes to study psychopathology is threefold: first, it reduces the complexity of symptoms and multifaceted behaviors by using quantitative units of measurement; second, a quantitative approach allows more power to detect linkage and is ideal for quantitative trait linkage analysis; third, relatively simpler phenomena involve fewer genes to produce functional abnormalities as compared to psychiatric diagnoses such as schizophrenia. Theoretically, the association between the endophenotype and the genes in question should be stronger than the association to the illness itself when discovered by candidate gene analysis (7, 8).

One endophenotype that is well represented in schizophrenia research is prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle (PPI). The startle reflex, easily measured in animals and humans, is mediated by a simple subcortical circuit (9). PPI is the phenomenon whereby a weaker non-startling “prepulse” stimulus presented shortly before the louder startling stimulus inhibits the magnitude of the startle reflex. PPI has been shown in numerous studies to be impaired in schizophrenia (10).

Another startle measure that has shown promise in recent findings is latency of the acoustic startle response. Startle latency is the time that it takes for the startling stimulus to travel through a three-synapse subcortical circuit that mediates the startle response, thus providing a putative index of neuronal processing speed. The finding of slower acoustic startle latency in schizophrenia than in controls has been reported in a number of studies (1015), although other papers reported no difference in latency between SCZ and controls (1627). However, the issue is more nuanced than simple between group differences. Most studies of acoustic startle in schizophrenia used a paradigm that measured PPI. For such sessions, latency facilitation is the characteristic shortening of startle latency in prepulse + pulse trials (in which a non-startling prepulse stimulus precedes the startling stimulus) compared to pulse-alone trials (trials without a prepulse). In some of the studies reporting no overall longer latency in the pulse-alone condition, the schizophrenia subjects had a lesser degree of latency facilitation than controls, thus displaying slower latency on prepulse + pulse trials (16, 22, 27). A heritability analysis by our group found that latency is highly heritable (68–90%) in pulse alone trials and is prolonged both in schizophrenia and family members of subjects with schizophrenia (13).

Importantly for the feasibility of genetic studies, the slowing of latency in subjects with schizophrenia can be detected in those who are treated with antipsychotic medications, as several studies report prolonged latency in schizophrenia cohorts all or most of whom were on antipsychotics (11, 15, 16, 22, 28). Similarly, in two prior published papers on this topic, latency was not affected by medication status (29, 30). However, it should be noted that Weike et al. (27) found differences in latency to prepulse + pulse trials in five unmedicated subjects compared to 20 medicated subjects. Prolonged startle latency is able to identify young subjects at clinical risk for schizophrenia even before they develop schizophrenia (31). Thus, latency shows promise as a potential biomarker of neural abnormalities in schizophrenia and a potential endophenotype. However, a slowing of latency is not unique to schizophrenia. It has been reported in autism (32, 33), Huntington’s disease (34), and post-traumatic stress disorder (25, 35). It is unknown how closely the neural underpinnings of slowed latency in schizophrenia may overlap with that of these other disorders.

Although several endophenotypes have thus far yielded positive findings in genetic association studies with schizophrenia (36), there have been very few studies that have looked at startle latency in relation to genetics relevant to schizophrenia. These studies have focused on single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in the human populations and knockout or knockdown genetics in the animal models. Studies in rodents indicate latency association with the RELN gene (37), which gene associates with SCZ in human studies (38). Four sites were associated with latency changes in mouse models, most notably FABP7 (39). There have been even less work looking at genetic variations in latency in human populations. Roussos et al. (40) report an association of slower latency with the DRD3 Ser9Gly polymorphism on SNP rs6280 in healthy humans. To our knowledge, there has not been a study of the genetics of startle latency in subjects with schizophrenia or psychosis. In this study, we conducted a gene-based analysis of startle latency, followed by a replication sample in which SNPs of significant candidate genes were examined.

Materials and Methods

Grady Trauma Project Sample

Subjects

The subjects in this cohort were part of a larger investigation of genetic and environmental factors that predict the response to stressful life events in a predominantly African American, urban population of low socioeconomic status (41). This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Emory University Institutional Review Board with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave their informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Emory University Institutional Review Board and the Grady Health Systems Research Oversight Committee.

Subjects were recruited from the medical, obstetrics-gynecology, and primary care clinic waiting rooms at Grady Hospital in downtown Atlanta, GA, USA. A diagnosis of previous and current mental illnesses was made using either the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV, Axis-I (SCID-I) (42), or the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI) (43). A history of past or current substance use was collected using the SCID-I, the Kreek–McHugh–Schluger–Kellogg (44), and the drug abuse screening test (45). Substance use disorders were classified by summary dichotomous variables based on combined information from the above listed instruments. Current drug use was coded based on current drug use items from these instruments and also a positive or negative urine toxicology that was collected on a subset of subjects. Subjects were classified as having other psychiatric diagnoses based on a combined variable that took into account both SCID-I and MINI data and classified them in a dichotomous variable coding for a history of a given diagnosis vs. no history of that diagnosis. Subjects were excluded if they had a history of sustained head trauma or seizure disorder, major neurologic or medical illness, or hearing impairment as ascertained by a hearing test conducted with an audiometer (Grason-Stadler, Model GS1710). The criterion for adequate hearing acuity was ability to discern tones in each ear of ≤40 dB [A] at 0.25, 0.5, 1, 2, 4, and 8 kHz.

Startle Magnitude and Latency Measurements

Subjects were tested in an acoustic startle session designed to assess startle magnitude and fear potentiation of startle. Subjects were seated in a sound attenuating booth during the test session. The eye blink component of the acoustic startle response was measured by electromyography (EMG) of the right orbicularis oculi muscle recorded with the Biopac MP150 system for Windows (Biopac Systems, Inc., Aero Camino, CA, USA) using methods previously published by our group (46). The EMG signal was recorded from two disposable Ag/Ag-Cl electrodes from Biopac positioned 1 cm below the right pupil and 1 cm below the right lateral canthus. Impedance levels were less than 6 kV for each participant as measured by a Checktrode impedance meter (1089 MKIII; UFI, Morro Bay, CA). The recorded EMG signal was sampled at a frequency of 1,000 Hz and was filtered with low frequency (28 Hz) and high frequency (500 Hz) cutoffs. The resultant data were then rectified and smoothed using the MindWare software (Mindware Technologies, Inc., Gahanna, OH, USA).

The acoustic startle probes were 40 ms 108 dB [A] SPL white noise bursts with near instantaneous rise time presented binaurally through Maico headphones (model TDH-39-P). The session began with four trials of noise alone startle stimuli to assess baseline startle magnitude, from which data on acoustic startle latency were extracted. Startle data obtained later in the session to assess fear potentiation were not used for the current analyses.

Startle magnitude for each trial was the maximum EMG signal occurring between 20 and 120 ms after the startling stimuli. Latency was the time of maximal EMG magnitude after the presentation of the startling stimuli (peak latency). Latency data were considered valid if the startle recorded did not begin before 20 ms after the startle stimulus and the startle magnitude was at least 15 µV.

DNA Extraction and Genetic Assays

The subjects provided a saliva or blood sample. DNA was extracted from saliva in Oragene collection vials (DNA Genotek Inc., ON, Canada) using the DNAdvance kit (Beckman Coulter Genomics, Danvers, MA, USA), while DNA from blood was extracted using either the E.Z.N.A. Mag-Bind Blood DNA Kit (Omega Bio-Tek, Inc., Norcross, GA, USA) or the ArchivePure DNA Blood Kit (5 Prime, Inc., Gaithersburg, MD, USA).

Genotyping was performed using the Omni-Quad 1M (Illumina, San Diego, CA, USA). Genotypes were called using the Illumina’s GenomeStudio software. We used PLINK to perform quality-control (QC) analyses on the genetic data. In brief, initial QC involved removing samples with very low call rates and those outside acceptable levels of heterozygosity (−0.25 < Fhet > 0.25); the remaining samples were recalled in GenomeStudio. We then removed SNPs with call rates less than or equal to 98%, a frequency of less than 0.01, and individuals with greater than 2% missing data. We further identified and removed related individuals by using PLINK to estimate the proportion of identity by descent (IBD) for each pair of individuals. Among pairs of individuals with an IBD proportion >0.12 (indicating cousins or a closer relation), we removed the individual in each pair with the higher rate of missing genotype data. Using data (autosomes only) pruned in PLINK, we performed principal-component analysis to infer axes of ancestry. Subjects that fell within 3 SDs of the medians of the first and second PCs in our sample were considered outliers and removed.

Statistical Analysis

PLINK was used to regress startle latency on allele count assuming an additive model (0, 1, or 2 copies of a risk allele), including sex, and the top 10 PCs of genome-wide data as covariates (47, 48). Gene-based association tests were performed using the minSNP method as implemented in FAST using the summary data derived from the SNP-based association tests described above (49). The gene-based p-value was calculated by using the permuted p-value of the best individual SNP association within the given gene. minSNP computes single SNP F-statistics for each SNP within a gene and uses the best F-statistic within that gene as its test statistic. The p-value was calculated using 1,000,000 permutations to correct for gene size (50). The precomputed haplotype and index files for the ASW population were downloaded from the FAST wiki page and used as a reference. The number of tests performed was calculated based on the number of genes analyzed (N = 32,194), and the false discovery rate (FDR) was controlled at 5% to account for multiple testing. Pathway analysis was performed with DAVID (51), and the FDR was controlled at 5%.

Independent Replication Sample of Veterans Affairs Subjects

Subjects

We next focused on these genes in a SNP-based replication study on the Atlanta Veterans Affairs Medical Center (VA) cohort, which consisted of 324 subjects, of whom 185 had schizophrenia and 139 were psychiatric controls. This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Emory University Institutional Review Board with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave their informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Emory University Institutional Review Board and the Atlanta Veterans Affairs Medical Center Human Subjects Committee.

We examined regressions on both onset latency (the time from presentation of the startling stimulus until the onset of the startle blink) and peak latency (the time from presentation of the startling stimulus until the time of peak magnitude of the startle blink). The subjects in this independent replication sample were collected through a VA funded Merit Review Project [PI, (29)]. The sample consisted of 185 unrelated schizophrenia subjects (SCZ) and 139 psychiatric control subjects (CON) with DNA extracted from blood samples and startle testing completed. Demographic data were collected on all subjects, and psychiatric medication status was collected on the SCZ group. Current symptom ratings were conducted on the SCZ subjects by means of the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (52). The DSM-IV diagnosis for SCZ subjects or lack thereof for CON subjects was confirmed based on the SCID-I (42). Female subjects were tested during the first 2 weeks of their menstrual cycle (follicular phase), because prior work indicates that women express a reduced prepulse inhibition during the luteal phase (53, 54).

Startle Magnitude and Latency Measurements

Hearing acuity was assessed by means of audiometer testing with a Grason-Stadler audiometer (Model GS1710). To be included, subjects had to detect tones in each ear at a threshold of 40 dB [A] at 0.25, 0.50, 1, 2, 4, and 8 kHz. Subjects completed a startle paradigm designed to assess magnitude, latency, and prepulse inhibition of startle using methods developed by Braff and colleagues (17) as described in prior publications from our group (13, 55). Briefly, subjects were seated in a sound attenuating booth and asked to look straight ahead and keep their eyes open during the testing. Acoustic stimuli were delivered binaurally through headphones (Maico, TDH-39-P). The eye blink component of the acoustic startle response was measured via EMG recording of the right orbicularis oculi muscle. Two Ag/Ag-Cl electrodes were positioned 1 cm below and right pupil and 1 cm below the right lateral canthus, with a ground electrode behind the right ear. The resistances for all subjects were less than 6 kΩ as measured with a Checktrode impedance meter (1089 MKIII; UFI, Morro Bay, CA, USA). EMG activity was amplified and digitized using a computerized EMG startle response monitoring system (SR-LAB, San Diego Instruments). The EMG signal was filtered with low- and high-frequency cutoffs at 30 and 1,000 Hz, respectively. The system was set to record 250 1-ms readings starting at the onset of the startle (pulse alone) stimulus. Digital signals were full-wave rectified and smoothed by an averaging routine that calculates a rolling average of 10 data points by means of SR-LAB analysis software.

The startle session began with a 60-s acclimation period consisting of 70 dB white noise that continued as background noise throughout the session. The pulse-alone stimuli were 116 dB, 40-ms bursts of white noise. The first block of the session consisted of six pulse alone stimuli. From this block, data on startle magnitude and latency were extracted for the current analyses. The next portion of the session, designed to assess prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle, was not used in the analyses presented herein.

DNA Extraction and Genetic Assays

To select genes for the focused replication analysis, we filtered the 2,870 significant genes from the gene-based analysis above based on whether they are expressed in the pons, a region of the brain that mediates startle response, and whether they have a prior association in the literature with startle (either latency or magnitude) or with schizophrenia or schizophrenia-related endophenotypes. This resulted in the following four genes that met these criteria: disrupted in schizophrenia 1 (DISC1), v-erb-b2 avian erythroblastic leukemia viral oncogene homolog (ERBB4), nitric oxide synthase 1 (neuronal) adapter protein (NOS1AP), and neuregulin (NRG1).

We proceeded with a SNP-based analysis of these four genes. Genotyping was performed in 384-well format using iPlex Gold kits and the Sequenom MassARRAY system. Amplification and extension primers were designed by SpectroDESIGNER software. The MassARRAY™ Typer software was used to assign the genotype calls. Each 384-well genotyping plate contained positive and negative controls. No samples were excluded based on missing data rates >5%. In total, 38 SNPs were genotyped of which 9 were eliminated because of call rates <95% and deviations from Hardy–Weinberg proportions (p ≤ 0.01), resulting in 29 SNPs that were analyzed for their relation with latency. Six SNPs had minor allele frequencies below 5%, and MAF was considered in interpretation of the results.

Statistical Analysis

We examined the association between latency and each SNP under one of the following two inheritance models: additive and dominant. For the additive model, each genotype was coded numerically, such that latency levels of heterozygotes should fall in between both homozygous groups. For those SNPs having a cell size for the minor allele less than 5, dominant models were evaluated assuming that a single copy of an allele is sufficient to influence the trait. We examined regressions on both onset latency (the time from presentation of the startling stimulus until the onset of the startle blink) and peak latency (the time from presentation of the startling stimulus until the time of peak magnitude of the startle blink). For each of these potential models, we examined association using linear regressions that incorporated covariates of age, race, and diagnosis (SCZ vs. CON). All p-values reported are two-sided.

Results

Gene-Based Analysis of Grady Sample

Demographic information on the Grady sample is listed in Table 1. Of the 380 subjects tested, 19 subjects were non-startlers and, therefore, did not have valid latency measures. An additional 48 were removed during GWAS QC. Therefore, the final analyses were conducted on 313 subjects. The mean age was 41.1 ± 12.1 years. 69.3% of the subjects were female and 30.7% were male. The population was primarily African American at 98.7%, with 1.3% being Caucasian. Additional diagnostic information and latency values for subgroups of subjects are presented in Table 1. No diagnostic subgroups had significant differences in peak latency by t-tests, as is noted in Table 1. Urine toxicology testing was available on a subset of Grady subjects (n = 48). Latency did not differ between those who were positive vs. negative (t = −0.87, p = 0.40). Additionally, those subjects admitting to current drug use on structured interview or having positive urine toxicology at the time of testing did not differ significantly in latency from those without current drug use or positive urines (t = −0.994, p = 0.32).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the final Grady sample (N = 313).

Gene-based associations for startle latency are summarized in Table 2. Consistent with the high degree of heritability for startle latency, 2,870 genes demonstrated evidence of association after correction for multiple comparisons (FDR < 0.05; Table S1 in Supplementary Material). Pathway analysis of these genes revealed enrichment for relevant biological processes including transmission of nerve impulses (p = 0.0029), synaptic transmission (p = 0.0032), and neuron development (p = 0.024) among others (Table 2). Figure 1 shows details of the gene-based analysis for four genes that were significant in this analysis after Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons, are expressed in the pons, and known to be associated with schizophrenia: DISC1, ERBB4, NOS1AP, and NRG1.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Biological processes enriched in pathway analysis of significant gene-based associations with startle latency.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Results of gene-based analyses of the Grady sample for: (A) DISC1, (B) ERBB4, (C) NOS1AP, and (D) NRG1.

Replication Study of Veterans Affairs Subjects

Table 3 displays demographic and clinical characteristics of this sample consisting of 185 subjects with schizophrenia and 139 psychiatric controls. Of this sample, 63 subjects were non-startlers and, therefore, did not have evaluable latency measures. Therefore, the final analyses were conducted on 277 subjects. This sample was more racially diverse and contained a greater percentage of males than the GTP sample. The SCZ sample consisted predominantly of outpatients stabilized on antipsychotic medication, although 15 subjects were unmedicated at the time of testing.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the final VA sample.

Table 4 shows the onset latency and peak latency values for this independent follow-up sample. Neither onset latency nor peak latency differed in SCZ or CON subjects according to sex. Regarding race, in CON only, the African American or other subjects had longer onset latency than Caucasian subjects. Onset latency and peak latency were significantly correlated in the VA sample as a whole (Pearson r = 0.560, p < 0.001), in the SCZ subjects (Pearson r = 0.56, p < 0.001), and in the CON subjects (Pearson r = 0.57, p < 0.001).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Latency values for the VA sample.

Table S2 in Supplementary Material provides details for the 29 SNPs that were tested on these four genes. Table 5 summarizes results for SNPs on the four genes selected for our independent follow-up study (DISC1, ERBB4, NOS1AP, and NRG1) that predicted slowing of latency (p < 0.05) in either an additive or dominant models. After Bonferroni correction of p = 0.0017 for multiple comparisons, only the finding for the NRG1 gene was significantly associated with startle latency. Our analysis revealed a strong association of onset latency with SNP rs901561 on NRG1, as can be seen in additive and dominant models shown in Figure 2. Subjects with the AA and AG genotypes of rs901561 (NRG1) had slower mean latency than subjects with the GG genotype (beta = 0.21, p = 0.001).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Association of single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) with startle latencya.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The single-nucleotide polymorphism rs901561 on the neuregulin gene (NRG1) predicted onset latency. Values are mean ± SEM. (A) Results of additive model (beta = 0.21, p = 0.001). p-Values shown are for post hocs. (B) Results of dominant model (beta = −0.15, p = 0.02). p-Value shown is for one-way ANOVA.

Two SNPs in DISC1 predicted slowing of latency, although the significance level did not survive our correction for multiple comparisons. Subjects with the AA genotype at rs2082552 had slower predicted latency than subjects with GG, with AG subjects intermediate (beta = 0.14, p = 0.03). The DISC1 SNP, rs3738402, significantly predicted peak latency in a dominant model (beta = 0.16, p = 0.01), with subjects with the CT or TT genotypes demonstrating slower latency than those with the CC genotype. For this SNP, there was only one TT homozygote, so an additive model could not be computed.

Two SNPs on ERBB4 also predicted latency, although the significance level did not survive our correction for multiple comparisons. Subjects with the GG genotype at rs10932374 had slower predicted onset latency than AA subjects, with AG subjects having intermediate latency (beta = −0.12; p = 0.05). Similarly, rs12467225 also predicted onset latency in an additive model (beta = 0.12; p = 0.05), with subjects that had the CC genotype exhibiting slower latency than TT subjects and CT subjects exhibiting intermediate latency.

Discussion

In this study, we used a gene-based analysis to examine the association of startle latency with multiple genes in a hypothesis neutral manner. This revealed associations with 2,870 genes involved in neural transmission, synaptic transmission, and neuronal development. We then conducted a SNP-based follow-up study on an independent sample targeting genes expressed in the pons, a region that mediates startle, and known from prior work to be associated with schizophrenia, schizophrenia endophenotypes, or acoustic startle. This SNP-based analysis revealed a significant association of startle latency with a SNP on the NRG1 gene, with two loci on the DISC1 gene, and with two loci on the ERBB4 gene, although these latter four loci did not survive Bonferroni correction.

The gene-based analysis revealed a significant association of >2,000 genes with startle latency. The preponderance of these genes is known to be involved in processes that could be expected to be relevant to startle latency, such as transmission of nerve impulses, synaptic transmission, and neuronal development. We have hypothesized that slowing of startle latency may reflect a more generalized slowing of neural transmission (13). Although it is currently not known which processes that subserve neural transmission may be affected in schizophrenia such that these individuals have slowed latency, the genetic findings that we report herein provide clues to the potential molecular underpinnings of slowed latency in schizophrenia.

Our analysis revealed a strong association of latency with the NRG1 gene in our gene-based analysis, and in our SNP-based replication, rs901561 was significant in both additive and dominant models. This gene has been reported as being associated with SCZ in large GWAS studies (56, 57) and confirmed by meta-analysis (58, 59), but the association with slowing of latency suggests that this SNP may identify a subset of individuals with the biomarker of slowed latency. It should be noted, however, that a prior study did not detect a significant association of a different SNP with startle latency in healthy controls (60). The gene is expressed in the pons (61), although the relationship between rs901561 and NRG1 remains to be determined. Neuregulins have frequently been associated with schizophrenia, as those deficient in NRG1 have fewer working NMDA receptors. NMDA receptors directly aid in glutamate transmission, and disruptions in glutamate signaling have been widely linked to schizophrenia (36, 56, 62). In the Greenwood et al. study (36, 63), NRG1 was significantly associated with several of their endophenotypes, including startle magnitude, prepulse inhibition of startle, verbal learning, abstraction, spatial processing, and sensorimotor dexterity. It may be that cognitive and motor performance impairments associated with NRG1 genotypes are in part mediated or modulated by slowing of neural processing as indicated by slowed startle latency. We have speculated that slowing of neural processing could lead to the dysfunction of neural circuits subserving cognitive or psychomotor performance, although to date the two studies examining startle latency and cognition have not found significant correlations (15, 64). However, a preliminary analysis of data from our lab has found a significant association between onset latency and cognitive function after adjusting for sex, race, age, and diagnosis (SCZ vs. CON; N = 221). In the Finger Tapping Task, a test of motor speed, we found that a higher score (i.e., more taps in a set period of time) corresponded with faster onset latency in both dominant (B = −0.038, p = 0.048) and non-dominant (B = −0.057, p = 0.014; unpublished data) hands.

It is worth noting the association of two ERBB4 SNPs with latency, rs10932374 and rs12467225, although the significance for these SNPs did not survive correction for multiple comparisons. ERBB4 has been extensively investigated as a vulnerability gene for schizophrenia (6567). It was included in the Consortium on Schizophrenia study and yielded significance for several endophenotypes, notably P50 gating, antisaccades, and several cognitive measures (36, 63). Neuregulin signaling via ERBB4 receptors has an important role in axonal guidance (68) and control of activity-dependent dendritic spines (69). The neuregulin-ERBB4 signaling pathway is emerging as being important in both glutamatergic and GABAergic neurotransmissions (70) [see review in Ref. (71)], and both these neurotransmitters are robustly implicated in the pathogenesis of schizophrenia (62, 72). The fact that our analysis revealed associations of latency with both NRG1 and ERBB4 renders these finding complementary and suggests an involvement of the neuregulin-ERRB4 signaling pathway as a potential route by which slowed latency, and neural slowing, could emerge in genetically vulnerable individuals.

It is also worth noting that our SNP-based analysis revealed significant associations of latency with two SNPs on the DISC1 gene, rs2082552 and rs3738402, although these associations were above the level of significance required by a Bonferroni correction. DISC1 is a known vulnerability gene for schizophrenia (7377). Several lines of evidence implicate the role of the DISC1 gene in the etiopathophysiology of SCZ (78, 79). The gene encodes a protein that modulates cortical growth and development, which could explain why mutations and malfunctions in the gene could increase latency and slow neuronal processing speed. DISC1 variants are associated with startle magnitude (63), P300 deficits (73, 80), sensory gating deficits, antisaccade deficits (36), and cognitive deficits (36, 78). In parallel with our current findings, prolonged latency of the P300 wave was associated with a DISC1 locus translocation (73), and the authors postulate that this P300 prolongation indicates slowed neural processing of the stimuli.

Our genetic findings for onset latency and peak latency in the VA sample were divergent, even though there was a robust correlation between onset latency and peak latency for the sample as a whole and for each subject group separately. The analyses on the Grady sample used peak latency. In the literature regarding latency differences between SCZ and CON, onset only was used in one study (27), peak was used in six studies (10, 12, 1416, 19), and both onset and peak were used in three studies (11, 13, 22). The reason that these two closely correlated measures yield different results is unclear at this time.

One strength of this study is the use of a gene-based analysis for discovery, with replication in an independent sample. Genetic associations can be difficult to replicate between cohorts due to differences in power, genetic ancestry, or clinical factors. Gene-based associations are more likely to replicate across populations because they aggregate signals across functional units of gene expression and have a lower threshold for multiple test correction (81, 82). Gene-based association tests complement traditional genetic approaches and have been used to identify additional genes associated with complex traits in previous studies (8386).

The study has several limitations. First, the study size limited the ability to detect significance when correcting for multiple tests. A Bonferroni correction for testing of 29 SNPs yields a corrected p-value of 0.0017, which significance threshold was not reached by several of our significant findings. A larger sample size would be ideal to adequately detect significance in many of the SNPs that were trending toward corrected significance. Additionally, because of the smaller sample size, many of the findings in the additive models could not be interpreted since the least frequent genotype was too small to be able to interpret the results. Second, in the SNP-based replication study, we looked at only a limited number of SNPs on the four genes that emerged from our gene-based analysis. It is possible that had we analyzed additional SNPs on these genes, additional significant results would have emerged. Third, the gene-based part of this study looked at a predominantly African American population and, thus, the results may not be generalizable to other populations. However, our replication study in the VA population consisted of a sample that was quite well balanced racially. Fourth, a sizable percent of the Grady subjects had current or past substance use disorders and 49 subjects either admitted to current drug use or had positive urine toxicologies. Latency was not significantly different in this currently using group than those without current drug use, but our findings would have been strengthened had urine testing been available on all Grady subjects.

In summary, the results of this study need replication in a larger data set, but the preliminary results support an association of startle latency with the multiple genes involved in neuronal signaling and development, including the NRG1 gene. A significant association of latency with a SNP on NRG1 was found in an independent subject sample. Evidence of longer (i.e., slower) acoustic startle latency as indirect evidence of slowing of neural processing supports the underlying hypothesis that genetically determined alterations of the neuregulin-ERBB4 and glutamate signaling pathways play a role in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. These results need replication in larger datasets, and future studies should be conducted on other genes that were significant in our discovery sample. However, our latency findings may provide a foundation from which we can characterize a distinct subgroup of schizophrenia patients and discover the neurobiology underlying the association of slowed latency with schizophrenia risk.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Emory University Institutional Review Board with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave their informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Emory University Institutional Review Board.

Author Contributions

AS, TJ, KR, and ED designed the experiments. AS, TJ, SN, BC, BB, KR, and ED performed the experiments. AS, TJ, VK, AL, LG, SL, NM, BB, KR, and ED analyzed the data. AS, LG, SL, and ED wrote the manuscript. All authors edited and approved the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

Infrastructure support was provided by Grady Hospital, Atlanta, GA, USA; the Research and Development and Mental Health Service Lines, Atlanta Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Decatur, GA, USA, and the Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences of the Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, GA, USA. ED is a full time attending psychiatrist, SN and BB are employees in the Mental Health Service Line, and NM is an employee of the Research and Development Service Line at the Atlanta Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Decatur, GA, USA. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health or the Department of Veterans Affairs.

Funding

This study was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (MH071537 and MH096764 to KR, MH092576 to TJ, and MH085806 to AS), the Veterans Affairs Merit Review Program (ED), the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number UL1TR000454, and an Emory University Research Committee Grant (URC grant to ED).

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at http://journal.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00117/full#supplementary-material.

References

1. Saha S, Chant D, McGrath J. A systematic review of mortality in schizophrenia: is the differential mortality gap worsening over time? Arch Gen Psychiatry (2007) 64(10):1123–31. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.64.10.1123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Mulle JG. Schizophrenia genetics: progress, at last. Curr Opin Genet Dev (2012) 22(3):238–44. doi:10.1016/j.gde.2012.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Sullivan PF, Daly MJ, O’Donovan M. Genetic architectures of psychiatric disorders: the emerging picture and its implications. Nat Rev Genet (2012) 13(8):537–51. doi:10.1038/nrg3240

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Ripke S, O’Dushlaine C, Chambert K, Moran JL, Kähler AK, Akterin S, et al. Genome-wide association analysis identifies 13 new risk loci for schizophrenia. Nat Genet (2013) 45(10):1150–9. doi:10.1038/ng.2742

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Ripke S, Neale BM, Corvin A, Walters JT, Farh KH, Holmans PA, et al. Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci. Nature (2014) 511(7510):421. doi:10.1038/nature13595

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Schizophrenia Psychiatric Genome-Wide Association Study (GWAS) Consortium. Genome-wide association study identifies five new schizophrenia loci. Nat Genet (2011) 43(10):969–76. doi:10.1038/ng.940

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Gottesman II, Gould TD. The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions. Am J Psychiatry (2003) 160(4):636–45. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.4.636

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Braff DL, Freedman R, Schork NJ, Gottesman II. Deconstructing schizophrenia: an overview of the use of endophenotypes in order to understand a complex disorder. Schizophr Bull (2007) 33(1):21–32. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbl049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Koch M. The neurobiology of startle. Prog Neurobiol (1999) 59(2):107–28. doi:10.1016/S0301-0082(98)00098-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Braff DL, Geyer MA, Light GA, Sprock J, Perry W, Cadenhead KS, et al. Impact of prepulse characteristics on the detection of sensorimotor gating deficits in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2001) 49(1–2):171–8. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(00)00139-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Braff DL, Swerdlow NR, Geyer MA. Symptom correlates of prepulse inhibition deficits in male schizophrenic patients. Am J Psychiatry (1999) 156:596–602. doi:10.1176/ajp.156.4.596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Csomor PA, Yee BK, Feldon J, Theodoridou A, Studerus E, Vollenweider FX. Impaired prepulse inhibition and prepulse-elicited reactivity but intact reflex circuit excitability in unmedicated schizophrenia patients: a comparison with healthy subjects and medicated schizophrenia patients. Schizophr Bull (2009) 35:244–55. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbm146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Hasenkamp W, Epstein MP, Green A, Wilcox L, Boshoven W, Lewison B, et al. Heritability of acoustic startle magnitude, prepulse inhibition, and startle latency in schizophrenia and control families. Psychiatry Res (2010) 178(2):236–43. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2009.11.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Storozheva ZI, Kirenskaya AV, Novototsky-Vlasov VY, Telesheva KY, Pletnikov M. Startle modification and P50 gating in schizophrenia patients and controls: Russian population. Span J Psychol (2016) 19:E8. doi:10.1017/sjp.2016.1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Swerdlow NR, Light GA, Cadenhead KS, Sprock J, Hsieh MH, Braff DL. Startle gating deficits in a large cohort of patients with schizophrenia: relationship to medications, symptoms, neurocognition, and level of function. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2006) 63(12):1325–35. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.63.12.1325

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Braff D, Stone C, Callaway E, Geyer M, Glick I, Bali L. Prestimulus effects on human startle reflex in normals and schizophrenics. Psychophysiology (1978) 15:339–43. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1978.tb01390.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Braff DL, Grillon C, Geyer MA. Gating and habituation of the startle reflex in schizophrenic patients. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1992) 49:206–15. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820030038005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Cadenhead KS, Swerdlow NR, Shafer KM, Diaz M, Braff DL. Modulation of the startle response and startle laterality in relatives of schizophrenic patients and in subjects with schizotypal personality disorder: evidence of inhibitory deficits. Am J Psychiatry (2000) 157:1660–8. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.157.10.1660

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Geyer MA, Braff DL. Habituation of the blink reflex in normals and schizophrenic patients. Psychophysiology (1982) 19:1–6. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1982.tb02589.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Kumari V, Soni W, Mathew VM, Sharma T. Prepulse inhibition of the startle response in men with schizophrenia: effects of age of onset of illness, symptoms, and medication. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2000) 57:609–14. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.57.6.609

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Leumann L, Feldon J, Vollenweider FX, Ludewig K. Effects of typical and atypical antipsychotics on prepulse inhibition and latent inhibition in chronic schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry (2002) 52:729–39. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01344-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Ludewig K, Geyer MA, Etzensberger M, Vollenweider FX. Stability of the acoustic startle reflex, prepulse inhibition, and habituation in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2002) 55:129–37. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(01)00198-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Ludewig K, Geyer MA, Vollenweider FX. Deficits in prepulse inhibition and habituation in never-medicated, first-episode schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry (2003) 54:121–8. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01925-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Mackeprang T, Kristiansen KT, Glenthoj BY. Effects of antipsychotics on prepulse inhibition of the startle response in drug-naive schizophrenic patients. Biol Psychiatry (2002) 52:863–73. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01409-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Massa NM, Duncan E, Jovanovic T, Kerley K, Weng L, Gensler L, et al. Relationship between Toxoplasma gondii seropositivity and acoustic startle response in an inner-city population. Brain Behav Immun (2017) 61:176–83. doi:10.1016/j.bbi.2016.11.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Parwani A, Duncan EJ, Bartlett E, Madonick SH, Efferen TR, Rajan R, et al. Impaired prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry (2000) 47(7):662–9. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00148-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Weike AI, Bauer U, Hamm AO. Effective neuroleptic medication removes prepulse inhibition deficits in schizophrenia patients. Biol Psychiatry (2000) 47(1):61–70. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(99)00229-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Hasenkamp W, Gross R, Lewison BA, Cuthbert B, Swails L, Duncan E. Latency of acoustic startle in schizophrenia: effects of antipsychotics. Neuropsychopharmacology (2014) 39:S456. doi:10.1038/npp.2014.281

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Duncan E, Szilagyi S, Schwartz M, Kunzova A, Negi S, Efferen T, et al. Prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle in subjects with schizophrenia treated with olanzapine or haloperidol. Psychiatry Res (2003) 120(1):1–12. doi:10.1016/S0165-1781(03)00161-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Duncan EJ, Szilagyi S, Efferen TR, Schwartz MP, Parwani A, Chakravorty S, et al. Effect of treatment status on prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle in schizophrenia. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (2003) 167(1):63–71. doi:10.1007/s00213-002-1372-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Cadenhead K, Addington J, Bearden C, Cannon T, Cornblatt BA, Mathalon DH, et al. Startle latency and magnitude predict clinical outcome in the psychosis prodrome: findings from the North American Prodromal Longitudinal Study (NAPLS). Neuropsychopharmacology (2013) 38(S2):S448–9. doi:10.1038/npp.2013.281

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Ornitz EM, Lane SJ, Sugiyama T, De Traversay J. Startle modulation studies in autism. J Autism Dev Disord (1993) 23:619–37. doi:10.1007/BF01046105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Takahashi H, Komatsu S, Nakahachi T, Ogino K, Kamio Y. Relationship of the acoustic startle response and its modulation to emotional and behavioral problems in typical development children and those with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord (2016) 46(2):534–43. doi:10.1007/s10803-015-2593-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Swerdlow NR, Paulsen J, Braff DL, Butters N, Geyer MA, Swenson MR. Impaired prepulse inhibition of acoustic and tactile startle response in patients with Huntington’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (1995) 58:192–200. doi:10.1136/jnnp.58.2.192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Siegelaar SE, Olff M, Bour LJ, Veelo D, Zwinderman AH, Van Bruggen G, et al. The auditory startle response in post-traumatic stress disorder. Exp Brain Res (2006) 174:1–6. doi:10.1007/s00221-006-0413-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Greenwood TA, Lazzeroni LC, Murray SS, Cadenhead KS, Calkins ME, Dobie DJ, et al. Analysis of 94 candidate genes and 12 endophenotypes for schizophrenia from the consortium on the genetics of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry (2011) 168(9):930–46. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10050723

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Tueting P, Costa E, Dwivedi Y, Guidotti A, Impagnatiello F, Manev R, et al. The phenotypic characteristics of heterozygous reeler mouse. Neuroreport (1999) 10(6):1329–34. doi:10.1097/00001756-199904260-00032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Shifman S, Johannesson M, Bronstein M, Chen SX, Collier DA, Craddock NJ, et al. Genome-wide association identifies a common variant in the reelin gene that increases the risk of schizophrenia only in women. PLoS Genet (2008) 4(2):e28. doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.0040028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Watanabe A, Toyota T, Owada Y, Hayashi T, Iwayama Y, Matsumata M, et al. Fabp7 maps to a quantitative trait locus for a schizophrenia endophenotype. PLoS Biol (2007) 5(11):e297. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0050297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Roussos P, Giakoumaki SG, Bitsios P. The dopamine D 3 receptor Ser9Gly polymorphism modulates prepulse inhibition of the acoustic startle reflex. Biol Psychiatry (2008) 64(3):235–40. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.01.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Gillespie CF, Bradley B, Mercer K, Smith AK, Conneely K, Gapen M, et al. Trauma exposure and stress-related disorders in inner city primary care patients. Gen Hosp Psychiatry (2009) 31(6):505–14. doi:10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2009.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. First M, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders. New York, NY: Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute (2001).

Google Scholar

43. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, et al. The Mini-international neuropsychiatric interview (MINI): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry (1998) 59(Suppl 20):22–33.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

44. Kellogg SH, McHugh PF, Bell K, Schluger JH, Schluger RP, LaForge KS, et al. The Kreek-McHugh-Schluger-Kellogg scale: a new, rapid method for quantifying substance abuse and its possible applications. Drug Alcohol Depend (2003) 69(2):137–50. doi:10.1016/S0376-8716(02)00308-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Skinner HA. The drug abuse screening test. Addict Behav (1982) 7(4):363–71. doi:10.1016/0306-4603(82)90005-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Jovanovic T, Blanding NQ, Norrholm SD, Duncan E, Bradley B, Ressler KJ. Childhood abuse is associated with increased startle reactivity in adulthood. Depress Anxiety (2009) 26(11):1018–26. doi:10.1002/da.20599

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Price AL, Patterson NJ, Plenge RM, Weinblatt ME, Shadick NA, Reich D. Principal components analysis corrects for stratification in genome-wide association studies. Nat Genet (2006) 38(8):904–9. doi:10.1038/ng1847

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Visscher PM. Handbook on analyzing human genetic data: computational approaches and software edited by Lin, S. and Zhao, H. Biometrics. (2010) 66:1310. doi:10.1111/j.1541-0420.2010.01510.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Chanda P, Huang H, Arking DE, Bader JS. Fast association tests for genes with FAST. PLoS One (2013) 8(7):e68585. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0068585

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Huang H, Chanda P, Alonso A, Bader JS, Arking DE. Gene-based tests of association. PLoS Genet (2011) 7(7):e1002177. doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.1002177

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Huang da W, Sherman BT, Lempicki RA. Systematic and integrative analysis of large gene lists using DAVID bioinformatics resources. Nat Protoc (2009) 4(1):44–57. doi:10.1038/nprot.2008.211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull (1987) 13:261–76. doi:10.1093/schbul/13.2.261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Jovanovic T, Szilagyi S, Chakravorty S, Fiallos AM, Lewison BJ, Parwani A, et al. Menstrual cycle phase effects on prepulse inhibition of acoustic startle. Psychophysiology (2004) 41:401–6. doi:10.1111/1469-8986.2004.00166.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Swerdlow NR, Hartman PL, Auerbach PP. Changes in sensorimotor inhibition across the menstrual cycle: implications for neuropsychiatric disorders. Biol Psychiatry (1997) 41:452–60. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(96)00065-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Hasenkamp W, Norrholm SD, Green A, Lewison B, Boshoven W, Keyes M, et al. Differences in startle reflex and prepulse inhibition in European-Americans and African-Americans. Psychophysiology (2008) 45:876–82. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00680.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Stefansson H, Sigurdsson E, Steinthorsdottir V, Bjornsdottir S, Sigmundsson T, Ghosh S, et al. Neuregulin 1 and susceptibility to schizophrenia. Am J Hum Genet (2002) 71(4):877–92. doi:10.1086/342734

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Stefansson H, Sarginson J, Kong A, Yates P, Steinthorsdottir V, Gudfinnsson E, et al. Association of neuregulin 1 with schizophrenia confirmed in a Scottish population. Am J Hum Genet (2003) 72(1):83–7. doi:10.1086/345442

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Li D, Collier DA, He L. Meta-analysis shows strong positive association of the neuregulin 1 (NRG1) gene with schizophrenia. Hum Mol Genet (2006) 15:1995–2002. doi:10.1093/hmg/ddl122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Munafo MR, Thiselton DL, Clark TG, Flint J. Association of the NRG1 gene and schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Mol Psychiatry (2006) 11:539–46. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001840

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Quednow BB, Schmechtig A, Ettinger U, Petrovsky N, Collier DA, Vollenweider FX, et al. Sensorimotor gating depends on polymorphisms of the serotonin-2A receptor and catechol-O-methyltransferase, but not on neuregulin-1 Arg38Gln genotype: a replication study. Biol Psychiatry (2009) 66:614–20. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Gibbs JR, van der Brug MP, Hernandez DG, Traynor BJ, Nalls MA, Lai SL, et al. Abundant quantitative trait loci exist for DNA methylation and gene expression in human brain. PLoS Genet (2010) 6(5):e1000952. doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.1000952

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Coyle JT. Glutamate and schizophrenia: beyond the dopamine hypothesis. Cell Mol Neurobiol (2006) 26:365–84. doi:10.1007/s10571-006-9062-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Greenwood TA, Lazzeroni LC, Calkins ME, Freedman R, Green MF, Gur RE, et al. Genetic assessment of additional endophenotypes from the Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia Family Study. Schizophr Res (2016) 170(1):30–40. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2015.11.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Hasenkamp W, Kelley M, Egan G, Green A, Wilcox L, Boshoven W, et al. Lack of relationship between acoustic startle and cognitive variables in schizophrenia and control subjects. Psychiatry Res (2011) 187:324–8. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2011.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Munafo MR, Attwood AS, Flint J. Neuregulin 1 genotype and schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull (2008) 34:9–12. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbm129

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Nicodemus KK, Luna A, Vakkalanka R, Goldberg T, Egan M, Straub RE, et al. Further evidence for association between ErbB4 and schizophrenia and influence on cognitive intermediate phenotypes in healthy controls. Mol Psychiatry (2006) 11(12):1062–5. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001878

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Silberberg G, Darvasi A, Pinkas-Kramarski R, Navon R. The involvement of ErbB4 with schizophrenia: association and expression studies. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet (2006) 141B:142–8. doi:10.1002/ajmg.b.30275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Lopez-Bendito G, Cautinat A, Sanchez JA, Bielle F, Flames N, Garratt AN, et al. Tangential neuronal migration controls axon guidance: a role for neuregulin-1 in thalamocortical axon navigation. Cell (2006) 125:127–42. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2006.01.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Li B, Woo RS, Mei L, Malinow R. The neuregulin-1 receptor erbB4 controls glutamatergic synapse maturation and plasticity. Neuron (2007) 54:583–97. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.03.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Woo RS, Li XM, Tao Y, Carpenter-Hyland E, Huang YZ, Weber J, et al. Neuregulin-1 enhances depolarization-induced GABA release. Neuron (2007) 54:599–610. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2007.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Balu DT, Coyle JT. Neuroplasticity signaling pathways linked to the pathophysiology of schizophrenia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2011) 35:848–70. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Lewis DA, Hashimoto T, Volk DW. Cortical inhibitory neurons and schizophrenia. Nat Rev Neurosci (2005) 6:312–24. doi:10.1038/nrn1648

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Blackwood DH, Fordyce A, Walker MT, St Clair DM, Porteous DJ, Muir WJ. Schizophrenia and affective disorders – cosegregation with a translocation at chromosome 1q42 that directly disrupts brain-expressed genes: clinical and P300 findings in a family. Am J Hum Genet (2001) 69:428–33. doi:10.1086/321969

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Ekelund J, Hovatta I, Parker A, Paunio T, Varilo T, Martin R, et al. Chromosome 1 loci in Finnish schizophrenia families. Hum Mol Genet (2001) 10:1611–7. doi:10.1093/hmg/10.15.1611

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Ekelund J, Hennah W, Hiekkalinna T, Parker A, Meyer J, Lonnqvist J, et al. Replication of 1q42 linkage in Finnish schizophrenia pedigrees. Mol Psychiatry (2004) 9:1037–41. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001536

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Hwu HG, Liu CM, Fann CS, Ou-Yang WC, Lee SF. Linkage of schizophrenia with chromosome 1q loci in Taiwanese families. Mol Psychiatry (2003) 8:445–52. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001235

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. St Clair D, Blackwood D, Muir W, Carothers A, Walker M, Spowart G, et al. Association within a family of a balanced autosomal translocation with major mental illness. Lancet (1990) 336:13–6. doi:10.1016/0140-6736(90)91520-K

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Porteous DJ, Thomson P, Brandon NJ, Millar JK. The genetics and biology of DISC1 – an emerging role in psychosis and cognition. Biol Psychiatry (2006) 60:123–31. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.04.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Palo OM, Antila M, Silander K, Hennah W, Kilpinen H, Soronen P, et al. Association of distinct allelic haplotypes of DISC1 with psychotic and bipolar spectrum disorders and with underlying cognitive impairments. Hum Mol Genet (2007) 16:2517–28. doi:10.1093/hmg/ddm207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Shaikh M, Hall MH, Schulze K, Dutt A, Li K, Williams I, et al. Effect of DISC1 on the P300 waveform in psychosis. Schizophr Bull (2013) 39:161–7. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbr101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Li MX, Gui HS, Kwan JS, Sham PC. GATES: a rapid and powerful gene-based association test using extended Simes procedure. Am J Hum Genet (2011) 88(3):283–93. doi:10.1016/j.ajhg.2011.01.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Liu JZ, McRae AF, Nyholt DR, Medland SE, Wray NR, Brown KM, et al. A versatile gene-based test for genome-wide association studies. Am J Hum Genet (2010) 87(1):139–45. doi:10.1016/j.ajhg.2010.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Jorgenson E, Witte JS. A gene-centric approach to genome-wide association studies. Nat Rev Genet (2006) 7(11):885–91. doi:10.1038/nrg1962

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Neale BM, Sham PC. The future of association studies: gene-based analysis and replication. Am J Hum Genet (2004) 75(3):353–62. doi:10.1086/423901

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Wang L, Jia P, Wolfinger RD, Chen X, Zhao Z. Gene set analysis of genome-wide association studies: methodological issues and perspectives. Genomics (2011) 98(1):1–8. doi:10.1016/j.ygeno.2011.04.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Kilaru V, Iyer SV, Almli LM, Stevens JS, Lori A, Jovanovic T, et al. Genome-wide gene-based analysis suggests an association between Neuroligin 1 (NLGN1) and post-traumatic stress disorder. Transl Psychiatry (2016) 24(6):e820. doi:10.1038/tp.2016.69

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: acoustic startle, latency, genetics, schizophrenia, endophenotype

Citation: Smith AK, Jovanovic T, Kilaru V, Lori A, Gensler L, Lee SS, Norrholm SD, Massa N, Cuthbert B, Bradley B, Ressler KJ and Duncan E (2017) A Gene-Based Analysis of Acoustic Startle Latency. Front. Psychiatry 8:117. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00117

Received: 05 January 2017; Accepted: 19 June 2017;
Published: 06 July 2017

Edited by:

Chad A. Bousman, University of Melbourne, Australia

Reviewed by:

Boris B. Quednow, University Hospital of Psychiatry, Switzerland
Jacob Raber, Oregon Health & Science University, United States
Esther Walton, University of Bristol, United Kingdom

Copyright: © 2017 Smith, Jovanovic, Kilaru, Lori, Gensler, Lee, Norrholm, Massa, Cuthbert, Bradley, Ressler and Duncan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Erica Duncan, erica.duncan@va.gov

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.