Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 25 September 2017
Sec. Psychopathology
This article is part of the Research Topic Cognitive Enhancement in Psychiatric Disorders View all 29 articles

Pharmacological Augmentation of Psychosocial and Remediation Training Efforts in Schizophrenia

  • 1Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of Miami Miller School of Medicine, Miami, FL, United States
  • 2Boehringer Ingelheim Pharmaceuticals, Inc., Ridgefield, CT, United States

Pharmacological approaches to cognitive enhancement have received considerable attention but have not had considerable success in improving their cognitive and functional targets. Other intervention strategies, such as cognitive remediation therapy (CRT), have been shown to enhance cognitive performance but have not been found to improve functional outcomes without additional psychosocial interventions. Recently, several studies have attempted to enhance the effects of CRT by adding pharmacological interventions to the CRT treatments. In addition, as CRT has been shown to synergistically improve the effects of psychosocial interventions, the combination of pharmacological therapies aimed at cognition and psychosocial interventions may itself provide a promising strategy for improving functional outcomes. This review and commentary examines the current state of interventions combining CRT and psychosocial treatments with pharmacological augmentation. Our focus is on the specific level of effect of the pharmacological intervention, which could be enhancing motivation, training efficiency, or the consolidation of therapeutic gains. Different pharmacological strategies (e.g., stimulants, plasticity-inducing agents, or attentional or alertness enhancers) may have the potential to lead to different types of gains when combined with CRT or psychosocial interventions. The relative potential of these different mechanisms for immediate and durable effects is considered.

Introduction

Cognitive impairments are prominent in several neuropsychiatric conditions (1). These impairments are functionally relevant and persistent over time and are minimally related to treatments for the illness (2). These impairments have spurred multiple treatment efforts spanning pharmacological, psychosocial, psychotherapeutic, and rehabilitation-based treatments. The rehabilitation-based treatments have used both in-person and computer-delivered cognitive remediation therapy (CRT) interventions (3). Furthermore, it has been suggested recently that combined pharmacological and cognitive remediation approaches may have the most promise for improving cognitive impairments in severe mental illness generally, and in schizophrenia specifically (4). However, much less attention has been paid to the potential combination of pharmacological treatments with psychosocial interventions. Pharmacological interventions may have the potential to synergistically combine with learning-based psychosocial treatments, much like the combination of these treatments with CRT training.

Previous reviews have analyzed the outcomes of both CRT and pharmacological interventions in schizophrenia and have demonstrated that the former is the more successful of the two. Clearly, cognitive remediation studies have demonstrated greater success, with several different studies finding effect sizes in the moderate to large range (3). In contrast, only a few studies have shown benefit for pharmacological treatments (5) and there is a lack of successful replication of these data and, to date, no positive phase III results have not been confirmed with larger samples in phase III studies (6). Thus, current research with pharmacological interventions have not demonstrated convincing ability to improve cognition as a monotherapy approach.

As the overarching goal of cognition-enhancing treatments is disability reduction, additional treatment approaches are needed to help achieve this. There have been multiple attempts to combine psychosocial interventions with CRT in order to enhance rehabilitation outcomes, but relatively fewer attempts to use the potential parallel strategy of combining targeted cognition-enhancing pharmacological therapies with psychosocial interventions. Most psychopharmacological and disability-targeted treatment data concern the pharmacological factors affecting interfere with the results of psychosocial interventions; however, this review focuses on the possibilities of additional therapeutic intervention.

In this article, we will focus on the combination interventions that could enhance cognitive performance and everyday functioning in people with schizophrenia. Encouraged by several very recent studies, we will examine the characteristics of CRT approaches that are suitable for combination with pharmacological interventions, as well as evaluating pharmacological interventions for their potential for combination with CRT, evaluating a model previously referred to as Pharmacologically Augmented Cognitive Training (PACT) (7). In addition to reviewing the possible benefits of combined CRT and pharmacological interventions, we also consider the possibility that combined pharmacological and psychosocial or psychotherapeutic interventions will have additional benefits when compared to either medication or behavioral intervention alone. The rationale for combining pharmacological cognitive enhancement with a psychosocial intervention is similar to that for adding CRT to a psychosocial intervention: using a synergistic therapeutic approach to enhance cognition through multiple strategies may enhance skill learning.

The evaluation of pharmacological strategies focuses on the mechanism of action, duration of effect, and potential impact on learning-based interventions such as cognitive remediation or behavioral interventions. This evaluation also necessitates a task analysis of CRT interventions, in terms of which cognitive processes are potentially important at different stages of the CRT participation process. Furthermore, available pharmacological interventions may interact with different stages of the CRT engagement process, which will refer to as performance-side variables.

An additional consideration when evaluating pharmacological augmentation of CRT is the fact that some augmentations may also increase the efficacy of CRT. For instance, if the rate of efficiency of training and gains across CRT levels was increased, it might also be the case that near transfer to neuropsychological test performance would be increased as well. There are several different ways that this increase could happen, including increased neuroplasticity (8) which could lead to greater beneficial brain changes with similar levels of effort and achievement.

Factors Influencing the Efficacy of CRT: Performance-Side Variables

Cognitive remediation therapy has been shown in multiple studies to improve performance on neuropsychological tests, although the efficacy of interventions varies across individuals (9). Several predictive factors for response to CRT have been identified, some of which may also be amenable to benefits from pharmacological interventions, as evaluated below.

There are several factors that can impact on the ability to perform in the training setting, and any of these factors can also influence gains during training. CRT differs from purely pharmacological interventions in that sustained effort-related participation on the part of the trainee is required. This level of effort is sustained for a training session that spans ≥30 min and thus, willingness and ability on the part of the trainee to participate in the training procedure is a prerequisite for training-related gains. While adherence to medication is clearly a participatory activity, the amount of effort expended, particularly in clinical treatment studies where medication is prepacked and delivered to the participant, seems less than that required to train for ≥30 min on a cognitively demanding task, ≥2 times/week.

Motivation

Motivation has been shown to exert an influence on cognitive changes associated with CRT (10). There are different types of motivation, including motivation induced or maintained by extrinsic factors and motivation arising from intangible and self-generated factors, which is often referred to as “intrinsic” motivation. For instance, several studies by Medalia et al. have documented a positive relationship between intrinsic motivation and CRT outcomes (1013). Intrinsic motivation refers to the willingness to perform the task because of the perception of benefit, rather than external rewards. The perception of intrinsic benefits is often augmented by certain elements of the CRT training process, such as bridging and discussion groups. It has been hypothesized that provision of external rewards for participating in CRT, such as financial compensation for training sessions, may lead to reduced transfer to other outcomes; however, it has recently been shown in a large-scale study that financial incentives to engage in behavioral rehabilitation programs do not lead to increases in clinician-rated engagement in treatment (14). In this study, individuals who showed high levels of motivation prior to treatment engagement demonstrated the greatest gains, while less motivated individuals did not show increased motivation, even with a potential to obtain considerable financial gain for increased treatment engagement.

Components of Motivation

Global Motivation to Participate in Therapeutic Activities

There are both global and specific components of motivation. Global motivation is the willingness to engage in an activity with the expectation that some benefit will be received. Thus, global motivation may require the ability to understand the means-ends relationships between engagement in remediation interventions and possible real-world gains. Individuals who are unaware that they have either cognitive or everyday functional deficits could therefore be expected to be less likely to be motivated to engage in treatment.

In several previous studies, it has been shown that individuals with schizophrenia who are unaware of their clinical symptoms, or functional or cognitive limitations, are also most likely to report that they have high quality of life and no mood symptoms. For instance, in the large-scale CATIE study, patients who reported that they were “pleased” or “delighted” with their quality of life were rated clinically as having less awareness of their illness (15). Those same patients also reported minimal depressive symptoms, but performed more poorly on tests of executive functioning than patients who had less positive self-assessment of their quality of life.

A recent study has shown the connection between insight and cognitive performance in patients with schizophrenia, as well as improvements in their cognitive performance with treatment. In this study, clinically unstable patients with schizophrenia were recruited as inpatients and treated with lurasidone, quetiapine, or placebo (16, 17). The outcomes measured included ratings of their clinical symptoms as well as performance-based assessments of cognition. At baseline, approximately 33% of patients had performance-based cognition scores that were invalid, presumably because of lack of adequate engagement or effort made in the assessment process. Interestingly, clinical ratings of poor insight into illness were more severe in patients whose test performance was invalid. After antipsychotic treatment, clinical insight improved, and there was a statistically significant improvement in cognitive performance in the trial which was found to be correlated with improvements in insight. Furthermore, patients who provided invalid test performance at baseline were able to generate valid scores after pharmacological treatment.

These data suggest that lack of awareness of impairment may be associated with the inability or unwillingness to engage in cognitive testing. CRT is considerably more demanding than a one-time cognitive assessment, and thus unawareness of impairment clearly has the potential to reduce global motivation to engage in a therapeutic activity. Although there is little research on this topic, these findings also suggest that clinical stability, including the presence of the awareness of clinical, cognitive, and functional status may be a prerequisite for success CRT. As a result, one pharmacological intervention strategy to combine with CRT may be the administration of adequate levels of antipsychotic medications in order to maintain clinical stability and thus potentially augment CRT results.

Specific Components of Motivation to Engage in Computerized Training

Cognitive remediation therapy also requires the willingness to interact repeatedly with technology, either in the form of computers or tablet devices. One of the reasons for the success of contemporary CRT interventions is the continuous feedback delivered to the trainee. As most CRT interventions use titrated difficulty, the feedback is positive about 80% of the time on a trial by trial basis. Furthermore, most programs also deliver prizes, messages, and other tokens of achievement, with the goal of feedback being to have the participant find the experience rewarding, hence increasing their level of engagement and motivation to continue to participate. Receipt of tokens and other awards can be considered an extrinsic reward; however, performance feedback also aims to increase the intrinsic motivation to continue to improve in performance.

A further form of encouragement is offered in the form of in-person activities associated with many CRT programs. These activities often take the form of bridging groups, which are meetings designed to make the CRT experience meaningful and useful in real-world cognitively demanding situations. These interventions further aim to offer social support for engagement in a largely technology-oriented intervention. The inclusion of these groups has been argued to be necessary for successful gains in CRT treatment (18). However, it may be the case that, in order to achieve real-world functional gains, the additional intervention needs to be highly relevant to everyday functionality and targeted at those skills. At least two recent studies have shown that CRT combined with bridging groups did not improve performance on either everyday outcomes or on measures of the acquisition of everyday functional skills (19, 20). In one of them (19), CRT plus bridging improved cognitive performance, but not functional capacity or everyday outcomes, whereas CRT plus bridging plus functional skills training improved cognitive performance, functional capacity, and everyday outcomes (20).

There are some illness-specific challenges in schizophrenia, for example, deficits in sensitivity to rewards, both extrinsic and intrinsic, are commonly reported in patients with schizophrenia and may underlie some of the “anhedonic” features of the condition (21). Deficits in social motivation, including active social avoidance, are also common in schizophrenia (22). These motivational deficits have been shown to be more closely tied to social outcomes than neurocognition, social cognition, or even social competence (2325). Thus, people with schizophrenia may not be sufficiently sensitive to rewards as unaffected individuals, and their social amotivation may also decrease their interest in bridging groups. In terms of extrinsic reward, it has been shown in several recent studies, reviewed by Gold et al. (26), that patients with schizophrenia are less responsive to financial reward for performing tasks than healthy comparison individuals, suggesting that reduced reward sensitivity applies to both tangible and intangible (i.e., social) reward systems.

It has been reported that self-administered CRT training has adherence rates of approximately 70% in both first episode psychosis, and the prodromal phases of the schizophrenia (27, 28). In these two studies, near transfer cognitive gains also appeared to be similar to previous studies that used in-person delivery of CRT, but not formal psychosocial interventions. Assessments to determine whether an individual with schizophrenia has high enough levels of active social avoidance to make coming in for treatment challenging may therefore be appropriate.

What is the Mechanism of Pharmacological Augmentation of CRT?

Pharmacological interventions have the potential to change motivation through modifying perceived reinforcement. For instance, stimulant medications such as amphetamine and related compounds (e.g., methylphenidate) have the potential to increase sensitivity to reward. As compounds that directly influence the dopamine-mediated reward system, they may increase the reward salience of CRT tokens and awards (29), as well as potentially increasing the sensitivity to treatment gains of an intrinsic nature. They may also increase intrinsic motivation through changing the salience of social interactions, but this is less well-studied than reward augmentation. These compounds also have the potential to improve attentional performance, which has the face-valid implication of increasing the ability to concentrate on training tasks. Further, processing speed may be augmented by these interventions, which can make it easier to make more rapid early gains and solidify motivation to perform.

PACT Results with Stimulant Medications: is Attention Augmented?

A very recent study has suggested that single-dose amphetamine treatment immediately prior to CRT training leads to increased gains in performance on the training test. Swerdlow et al. treated patients with schizophrenia with 10 mg of amphetamine or placebo in a double blind cross-over design (30). A test for auditory attention processing was administered around before and after 60 min of auditory training. Compared to placebo, these data suggested that amphetamine treatment had a substantial benefit on gains during auditory training, suggesting that session by session administration of cognition-enhancing compounds can lead to greater attentional gains with CRT. Furthermore, the benefit of treatment persisted one week after a single 1-h training session, an effect not seen with placebo.

However, this study does not address the other hypothesis mentioned above; that stimulant treatment may enhance the reward salience of CRT tokens and awards, which could lead to sustained motivation to engage in the task. Also, the motivation to engage in treatment due to perception of long-term gains might also be changed through interventions that increase potential sensitivity to the need to engage in productive activities such as work. This is clearly an important research topic and would require additional research efforts.

Other compounds related to stimulants have also been examined for their augmentation potential. Most commonly, modafinil, an alertness promoting agent, has been studied for both direct cognitive benefits and for augmentation of PACT. The results from modafinil studies have been complex, in that several single-dose studies have reported positive effects of treatment with modafinil as a monotherapy (31), while several multiple dose studies did not find similar efficacy. In a recent 10-session PACT study comparing a standard dose of 200 mg of modafinil to placebo, with the MATRICS consensus cognitive battery (MCCB) as the outcome measure, there was no augmentation effect on CRT (32). Both groups improved in performance, but the performance of the group receiving only CRT was not so substantial that it could have led to a ceiling effect that obscured the effects of modafinil augmentation.

It is of interest, therefore, whether the results of Swerdlow et al. (30), which examined a single dose of amphetamine vs. placebo, would result in the same lack of efficacy if a multiple dose strategy was employed. If the single-dose effect habituates over time, then this would not be an effective intervention. Similarly, no studies have yet examined the consequences of daily dosing with stimulant-like medications on response to CRT interventions. Clearly, this is an area where more research would be important, particularly in terms of the relative benefit of single-dose, multidose day of training, and daily dosing regimens.

Other Pharmacological Augmentation Strategies

Cognitive remediation therapy has often been described as an intervention which promotes neuroplasticity. Changes in both brain structure and function are commonly noted after CRT interventions, with changes in white matter structure and cognitive activation in response to cognitive stimuli (33). In addition, changes in serum levels of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) have been detected following CRT interventions (34). These changes approach full normalization of BDNF in cases treated with active CRT, while cases who participated in video game control treatment did not show these changes, despite equivalent levels of cognitive activity during the treatment period.

Pharmacological compounds too have the potential to lead to changes consistent with neuroplasticity. Compounds affecting the glutamatergic system have been cited as increasing brain plasticity response to various cognitive interventions in animal models (35). These interventions include increases in the rate of learning new information and the extent to which new information is rapidly consolidated in an adaptive manner. Much of this research has focused on facilitation of novel object recognition and maze learning paradigms (36). Both of these processes are also affected by antagonists at glutamatergic sites, including PCP and ketamine (37).

Importantly for this review, glutamatergic agents have been studied extensively in humans in other conditions because of their potential for modifying the learning process. In particular, d-cycloserine (DCS), initially used to consolidate fear extinction gains (38), has been studied for its potential to modify the memory consolidation process (39). This process is hypothesized to occur as a result of partial agonistic effects of DCS on N-methyl-d-aspartate glutamatergic receptors. This area of investigation has been challenging because, as DCS causes memory consolidation, it can cause consolidation of both the extinction of the older memories and consolidation of memory recurrence as well (40). Thus, DCS seems at present to have a role in exposure interventions, but the treatment needs to be carefully administered.

There have been multiple studies of the direct effects of various glutamatergic agents on cognition. These results have been consistently negative when using DCS, cycloserine, glycine, and glycine transport inhibitors to improve cognition directly (41, 42). However, this does not mean that these compounds would not have a beneficial effect when combined with CRT interventions.

To date, there has been one study that used DCS in concert with CRT (43), which treated patients with schizophrenia with 50 mg of DCS vs. placebo once weekly, 60 min prior to receiving a CRT session. There was a target of 3 CRT sessions per week over a planned 8-week trial and participants completed an average of 26 training sessions. The results of the trial indicated that participants who were treated with DCS showed improvement on the tasks in the CRT training procedure, but not on external cognitive outcomes, as measured by MCCB, which improved in the placebo group but not the active treatment group. Furthermore, DCS improved negative symptoms in patients who had clinically significant symptoms of this type at baseline, although there was no connection between cognitive gains and negative symptom improvements. Thus, the results of this study are complex; DCS appeared to enhance performance on the training tests, but the fact that patients treated with placebo had greater near transfer of training to cognitive test performance suggests that DCS may interfere with the commonly found transfer of CRT gains to cognitive performance. While improving negative symptoms is an important goal of treatment, this study suggests that improving motivation-related symptoms does not necessarily lead to better ability to engage in cognitive training procedures. The interference with transfer to untrained tests also suggests that DCS may not be the optimal strategy to promote transfer, in terms of both near transfer to cognitive test performance, and far transfer to real world functioning.

An additional study examined the effects of a related pharmacological compound, d-serine, on augmentation of CRT in schizophrenia (44). In this study, it was found that augmentation of CRT treatment did not lead to incremental benefits. However, the authors reported good safety outcomes and suggested that a higher dose may be required.

Conclusion on PACT

Pharmacological augmentation of CRT has been attempted and some success has been reported for certain study designs, primarily those using stimulant-like drugs. However, daily dosing studies have not yet been published yet, and studies of interventions aimed at increasing the potential neuroplasticity effect of CRT have been less successful to date. Furthermore, all of the possible dosing strategies and augmentation possibilities have not been explored. At this interim stage of the research process, the reasonable conclusion is therefore that this is a developing research area and more studies will most likely be reported in the immediate future which will hopefully enable firmer conclusions to be drawn.

Psychosocial Rehabilitation and the Interface with Cognitive Enhancement

Psychosocial rehabilitation efforts can be targeted at multiple domains of disability. The two main areas of focus in the past have been social skills (often referred to as social competence) and vocational outcomes, which have both reported some success with psychosocial rehabilitation. For instance, supported employment programs using an individualized placement and support (IPS) model have found that participants who received high quality services had a rate of obtaining competitive employment of approximately 40%, compared to 10–15% for patients who received standard psychiatric rehabilitation services (45). For social skills training, a Cochrane systematic review suggested that average intervention was not more effective than discussion groups for improving social functioning, relapse rates, mental state or quality of life (46). As social cognition is a critical component of social outcomes, there have been multiple attempts to train social skills. These interventions have had several forms, including training focused on social interactions [Social Cognition Intervention Training (SCIT)] and computerized training interventions. Results of studies of SCIT have suggested that there is some moderate benefit on performance-based measures of social cognition, particularly measures of hostile interpersonal interactions, but minimal effects on real-world social outcomes (4750). Computerized interventions have also shown some promise in terms of improving performance on social cognition measures. For instance, training on the Mind Reading: An Interactive Guide to Emotions (MRIGE) program improved social cognitive performance in individuals with an autism spectrum condition (51).

However, it is clear from the results of psychosocial interventions aimed at social functioning and vocational outcomes that, at most, half of the patients treated show benefit. Interestingly, it has also been shown that when compared to psychosocial interventions alone, patients who receive combined cognitive remediation and psychosocial interventions make more substantial and rapid gains. For instance, McGurk et al. (52) added approximately 20 sessions of CRT to an IPS model vocational intervention and found employment gains that were persistent for 3 years. In a follow-up study, McGurk et al. (53) also found that adding CRT to IPS in IPS non-responders led to a rapid and sustained improvement in employment outcomes, this demonstrating that augmenting cognitive functioning can lead improve the response to ongoing psychosocial intervention.

This CRT enhancement effect appears robust across various psychosocial interventions. Bowie et al. (20) compared monotherapy with skills training using the Functional Adaptation Skills Training (FAST) model developed by Patterson et al. (54) or CRT alone to a combined FAST and CRT intervention, finding that the combined group had significantly greater gains in everyday functioning outcomes than both other treatments, as rated by blinded observers. FAST or CRT as a monotherapy led to domain-specific gains (functional skills and cognition, respectively) but no psychosocial improvements. These results suggest that CRT interventions can successfully combine with skills training interventions that are broadly aimed at functional skills in social, residential, and vocational domains.

Finally, Lindenmayer et al. (55) examined the combination of computerized social cognition training (MRIGE) and CRT compared with CRT alone on changes in performance-based assessments of social cognition and clinician ratings of clinical symptoms and social outcomes in patients with schizophrenia. Their study did not include a monotherapy social cognition arm, as their interest was in whether CRT improved social cognition. Their results indicated that the combined therapy lead to greater gains in performance on social cognitive tests as well as more gains in everyday social functioning without having a symptomatic benefit. Combining MRIGE and CRT did not dilute the effects of CRT on composite cognitive performance, which was significantly improved from baseline in both groups.

Cognitive Behavior Therapy (CBT) as an Augmentation Target

Another domain of psychosocial interventions aimed at improving symptoms of schizophrenia is that of CBT. CBT interventions are commonly targeted at treatment-refractory delusions or hallucinations. By their definition, CBT interventions require cognitive capacity for efficacy, and as a result, the substantial cognitive impairments seen in schizophrenia, even more salient in patients with evidence of clinical treatment resistance that would lead to CBT intervention, would seem to mitigate against the benefits of a learning-based therapy in patients with major learning problems. In fact, despite evidence of efficacy and considerable enthusiasm for CBT on the part of many proponents, the number needed to treat is higher than that for many pharmacological interventions (56).

Given that CBT interventions are targeted at populations selected for treatment-resistance and increased cognitive impairments, augmentation with CRT aimed at cognition would seem a viable strategy. Interestingly, there seems to be only one published study where CRT was combined with CBT in patients with severe mental illness (57). In that study, patients randomized to CRT prior to treatment with CBT had a more rapid response to CBT than cases randomized to other psychosocial interventions. A similar strategy could also be employed with pharmacological interventions. It would be straightforward to perform a randomized trial combining potential cognition-enhancing medications with CBT in order to see whether there was either faster response or increased benefit.

Augmentation of Psychosocial Training with Pharmacological Interventions

The substantial successes recorded from the combination of CRT and psychosocial interventions raise the question as to whether pharmacological augmentation strategies could lead to similar gains. While most pharmacological strategies have not had success on their own in terms of enhancement of cognitive performance, the combination of pharmacological interventions with CRT described above has led to incremental gains in some studies. Psychosocial interventions are themselves cognitively active and the possibility exists that synergistic effects could be seen with the combination of pharmacological augmentation of psychosocial interventions.

One interesting study has suggested that pharmacological factors may be critical for skills training interventions (58). It has been known for years that anticholinergic treatment of patients with schizophrenia is correlated with memory impairments. Memory impairments are functionally relevant, being one of the impairments most strongly correlated with everyday functional deficits. Even more important is the finding that anticholinergic medication levels correlate with the efficacy of CRT interventions. Specifically, Vinogradov et al. reported that serum levels of anticholinergic medications shared 20% of the variance with improvements in cognitive performance associated with CRT training. Thus, higher levels of anticholinergic medication can lead to more than just cross-sectional cognitive impairments, they can actually constrain the extent to which CRT provides a beneficial effect.

In another, recent study, similar effects were found for the influence of anticholinergic treatment and psychosocial treatments. Seventy patients with schizophrenia enrolled in psychosocial interventions were followed for 3 years (59). Total anticholinergic burden was assessed and patients were examined for cognitive performance and for progress in their psychosocial intervention programs. Anticholinergic burden predicted cognitive performance on the MCCB, which in turn predicted progress in rehabilitation. Clinical symptoms, antipsychotic treatments, and baseline level of functioning did not add variance to the MCCB scores for prediction of rehabilitation outcome. These data provide convincing evidence for the direct adverse effects on skills training of anticholinergic medications.

Procholinergic Treatments for Augmentation of Skills Training

Directly in line with the idea that that the cholinergic system may be critical for successful CRT and psychosocial intervention is the idea that procholinergic treatments may be a reasonable pharmacological enhancement strategy. There are two different approaches to procholinergic treatment: treatment with compounds that affect the muscarinic cholinergic system and others that target the nicotinic cholinergic system. Muscarinic targets have included M1 agonists and, much more commonly, acetylcholinesterase inhibitors (AChEIs). Nictonic targets have included a variety of partial agonist strategies aimed at the α-7 receptor with a smaller number of studies targeting a different receptor complex the α4β2.

Results from studies of the direct effect of cognitive enhancement with AChEI have been consistent and disappointing. Three different AChEI (donepezil, galantamine, and rivastigmine) have shown preliminary success in small scale studies, but larger studies have been consistently negative (6062). In fact, in one study, placebo treatment was superior to active treatment with donepezil (62). Another large-scale study examining galantamine (61) showed some that there were some domains of cognition that were potentially beneficially affected, but the overall effects of treatment on the predetermined cognitive outcomes were negative.

There have been other studies in patient with schizophrenia using the M1 agonist xanomeline (63). Although the compound appears to have efficacy for cognitive performance, its original manufacturer stopped its development because of significant gastrointestinal distress which lead to a substantial rate of discontinuation. At the present time, there are efforts to bypass the toxic effects of M1 agonist compounds by attempting to deactivate the mechanisms responsible for some of the side effects of the treatment.

Results with α4β2 treatments have also been reported. In a substantially powered randomized trial, the α4β2 compound varenicline was found to work as an effective smoking-cessation treatment, but did not have any detectable cognitive benefits, as measured by MCCB (64).

There has been much a more substantial effort in the domain of α-7 receptor agonists; however, results have been inconsistent. Several different treatments have failed to show differences from placebo in studies on patients with schizophrenia (65, 66). A short-term study of the α-7 receptor agonist DMXB-A found separation between active and placebo treatment in a cross-over design (67). However, a longer study with more participants found no beneficial effects of treatment (68). There were reports of substantial successes in a phase II clinical trial, which also had suitable coprimary measures (69). However, this drug also failed to separate from placebo in a much larger-scale phase III study of patients with schizophrenia (Hilts et al., in preparation). Finally, in a much smaller study, the mixed α-7/α4β2 receptor agonist tropisetron was reported to improve cognition in three different samples of 10 patients with schizophrenia compared with placebo (70). However, given the repeated failures of larger studies in this research area to replicate the results of smaller studies and the fact that tropisetron has been in clinical use for two decades for smoking cessation, a much larger confirmatory study will be needed.

The failures of procholinergic agents as monotherapy for cognition in schizophrenia do not necessarily mean that they would not be effective in improving the efficacy of either CRT or psychosocial interventions. Indeed, there is a precedent for a specified combination of a pharmacological agent and a psychosocial intervention: varenicline as an adjunct to clinical interventions for smoking cessation. Although varenicline may lead to reduced smoking in some populations, it has not been approved as a monotherapy. Thus, it is entirely possible that a combination of a pharmacological agent not approved on its own and either a CRT or psychosocial intervention could happen in the future.

Testing such interventions would seem to be a priority but may be challenging while funding agencies insist that all interventions targeted at severe mental illness have an identifiable, separable, and discretely measurable target to engage. In fact, the most recent developments in the “precision medicine” initiative of the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) have argued for an identifiable, single target and clear specifications of what constitutes a negative result. While neuropsychological tests can be easily specified as the targets for both pharmacological agents and CRT, the “target” for skills training may be more difficult to define. While Bowie et al. (20) used performance on a measure of functional capacity (the UPSA-B) as the index of treatment gains associated with a highly specific skills training program, vocational interventions are more challenging. While no one would argue that increases in hours worked or money earned is not important, it easy to criticize on the basis that there are multiple potential mechanisms of influence that could move these outcomes in a positive direction. Thus, the combination of pharmacological interventions with broader psychosocial interventions such as IPS is outside the realm of NIMH support at this time. A test of whether pharmacological mechanisms combined with IPS may therefore have to wait for the approval of a medication for cognitive impairment associated with schizophrenia and funding by the owner of the medication.

Other Pharmacological Augmentation Strategies

There have been several other domains of pharmacological interventions aimed at cognition in the schizophrenia spectrum, including interventions targeted at subclasses of monoamines, including dopamine D1 receptors and norepinephrine. D1 receptors have been an interesting target since animal work conducted by Patricia Goldman-Rakic et al. in the 1990s. Specifically, the D1 receptor agonist SKF 38393 was shown in several studies to have a direct beneficial effect on cognition in several animal models (71), although this compound does not cross the blood brain barrier in adequate concentrations to be useful for pharmacological augmentation. Other D1 agents, including dihydrexadine (DHX), have been tested in clinical trials. Although DHX did not lead to significant improvement in cognition in patients with schizophrenia (72), it was found to lead to significant improvements in working memory in patients with schizotypal personality disorder who had never been treated with antipsychotic medications (73). Broad spectrum dopamine agents such as pergolide (74) and pramipexole (75) have been examined in patients with schizotypal personality disorder and schizophrenia, respectively. However, pergolide has since been removed from the market because of adverse events, and pramipexole did not show remarkable efficacy in schizophrenia although it does not appear to have been tested in schizotypal personality disorder.

Similar findings were reported for the noradrenergic alpha-II agonist guanfacine, a currently approved treatment for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder found to improve cognition in animal models (76). In a clinical trial for schizophrenia, guanfacine did not improve cognition (77), but when used to treat patients with schizotypal personality disorder there was a significant positive treatment effect (78). Other noradrenergic interventions have also provided promising data, but have shown limited clinical efficacy in schizophrenia. For example, Friedman et al. (79) found that atomoxetine, a norepinephrine transport inhibitor, improved regional blood flow in critical areas compared to placebo in patients with schizophrenia; however, the cognitive benefits of the treatment did not achieve statistical significance. Kelly et al. (80) performed a 32-patient randomized trial and also did not find separation from placebo. However, all of these interventions have been tested only as monotherapy, without additional CRT or psychosocial interventions. The fact that cognition did not improve with these treatments does not mean that they would not manifest an incremental efficacy boost to these other learning-based interventions. As all of these treatments have demonstrated safety and tolerability, studies of combined therapy with these pharmacological strategies and CRT or psychosocial interventions would be able to proceed without many concerns about safety.

Conclusion

Combining pharmacological interventions with CRT has been suggested as a promising way forward for improving cognition in schizophrenia and has been previously tested in randomized research trials. There are several different levels at which cognition-enhancing drugs could beneficially impact on the results of CRT, including making it easier for participants to engage in CRT (performance-side variables) as well as augmenting the extent to which these interventions have a benefit (increasing plasticity). Furthermore, a larger cognitive gain with combined therapy might also be more likely to lead to functional gains without the requirement for psychosocial interventions. Despite the substantial gains seen with some CRT interventions, including average effect sizes of d = 0.8, patients in those trials still have substantial cognitive deficits at the end of the study. If those effect sizes could be doubled, then cognitive performance could be normalized, which might have implications for whether additional skills training would necessarily be required.

The results of studies combining pharmacological and CRT interventions have been mixed, as a function of the mechanism of the pharmacological add-on strategy. It is possible that the research designs employed in unsuccessful studies were not optimal, such as those including single-dose treatments, medications that may actually impair learning or transfer of information, or concerns regarding the dosing of the CRT interventions delivered.

Much less mixed are the results of combining cognitive enhancement induced by CRT with psychosocial interventions. These studies have consistently found augmented rates of acquisition of psychosocial target outcomes in cases who received also CRT interventions. Finally, although in its infancy, another area where cognition-enhancing interventions could be explored is their potential ability to facilitate clinical gains in CBT, as per the single study that suggested CRT may lead to an incremental benefit for CBT. This would obviously be an area where a combined trial design could employ a pharmacological cognitive enhancer instead of CRT as a cognitive enhancement strategy to boost the efficacy of CBT.

We suggest that similar research strategies could also be employed with pharmacological augmentation, using a research design where a putative cognition-enhancing compound was added to psychosocial interventions in a randomized trial. There are currently limited data regarding this, but there are many promising agents to test. The pragmatics of pharmacological augmentation by-pass the effort and motivation-related limitations associated with some CRT interventions. This is a largely unexplored area, but the synergistic effect of psychosocial interventions and pharmacological treatment strategies seems worthy of exploration.

A final consideration is whether the triple combination of pharmacological, CRT, and psychosocial interventions would be superior to the results of combining CRT and psychosocial interventions alone. If pharmacological augmentation of CRT is actually beneficial and increases gains compared to CRT alone, then this combination might lead to even greater functional gains when combined with psychosocial interventions. This is, of course, a research question that can be addressed directly.

Author Contributions

Both authors contributed equally to this article.

Conflict of Interest Statement

In the past year, PH has served as a consultant to Allergan, Boehringer Ingelheim, Lundbeck, Otsuka Digital Health, Sanofi, Sunovion, and Takeda. MS is a full-time employee of Boehringer Ingelheim.

Acknowledgments

Editorial assistance was provided by Michelle Marvel of Fishawack Communications, funded by Boehringer Ingelheim.

Funding

Boehringer Ingelheim paid the open-access fee for this article. No other funding was involved.

References

1. Harvey PD, Reichenberg A, Bowie CR. Cognition and aging in psychopathology: focus on schizophrenia and depression. Annu Rev Clin Psychol (2006) 2:389–409. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.2.022305.095206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Bowie CR, Harvey PD. Cognition in schizophrenia: impairments, determinants, and functional importance. Psychiatr Clin North Am (2005) 28(3):613–33, 26. doi:10.1016/j.psc.2005.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Wykes T, Huddy V, Cellard C, McGurk SR, Czobor P. A meta-analysis of cognitive remediation for schizophrenia: methodology and effect sizes. Am J Psychiatry (2011) 168(5):472–85. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10060855

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. McGurk SR, Mueser KT, Watkins MA, Dalton CM, Deutsch H. The feasibility of implementing cognitive remediation for work in community based psychiatric rehabilitation programs. Psychiatr Rehabil J (2017) 40(1):79–86. doi:10.1037/prj0000257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Harvey PD. Pharmacological cognitive enhancement in schizophrenia. Neuropsychol Rev (2009) 19(3):324–35. doi:10.1007/s11065-009-9103-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Keefe RS, Davis VG, Spagnola NB, Hilt D, Dgetluck N, Ruse S, et al. Reliability, validity and treatment sensitivity of the Schizophrenia Cognition Rating Scale. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol (2015) 25(2):176–84. doi:10.1016/j.euroneuro.2014.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Swerdlow NR. Beyond antipsychotics: pharmacologically-augmented cognitive therapies (PACTs) for schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology (2012) 37(1):310–1. doi:10.1038/npp.2011.195

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Goff D. The therapeutic role of D-cycloserine in schizophrenia. Adv Pharmacol (2016) 76:39–66. doi:10.1016/bs.apha.2016.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Scheu F, Aghotor J, Pfueller U, Moritz S, Bohn F, Weisbrod M, et al. Predictors of performance improvements within a cognitive remediation program for schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res (2013) 209(3):375–80. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2013.04.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Saperstein AM, Medalia A. The role of motivation in cognitive remediation for people with schizophrenia. Curr Top Behav Neurosci (2016) 27:533–46. doi:10.1007/7854_2015_373

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Choi J, Medalia A. Intrinsic motivation and learning in a schizophrenia spectrum sample. Schizophr Res (2010) 118(1–3):12–9. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2009.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Medalia A, Revheim N, Casey M. Remediation of memory disorders in schizophrenia. Psychol Med (2000) 30(6):1451–9. doi:10.1017/S0033291799002913

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Medalia A, Saperstein A. The role of motivation for treatment success. Schizophr Bull (2011) 37(Suppl 2):S122–8. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbr063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Kotwicki RJ, Balzer AM, Harvey PD. Measuring and facilitating client engagement with financial incentives: implications for improving clinical outcomes in a mental health setting. Community Ment Health J (2017) 53:501–9. doi:10.1007/s10597-016-0053-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Siu CO, Harvey PD, Agid O, Waye M, Brambilla C, Choi WK, et al. Insight and subjective measures of quality of life in chronic schizophrenia. Schizophr Res Cogn (2015) 2(3):127–32. doi:10.1016/j.scog.2015.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Harvey PD, Siu CO, Hsu J, Cucchiaro J, Maruff P, Loebel A. Effect of lurasidone on neurocognitive performance in patients with schizophrenia: a short-term placebo- and active-controlled study followed by a 6-month double-blind extension. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol (2013) 23(11):1373–82. doi:10.1016/j.euroneuro.2013.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Agid O, Siu C, Harvey PD, Zipursky Z, Fervaha G, Foussias G, et al. Treatment outcomes, insight, and recovery in first episode schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2014) 153:S165–6.

Google Scholar

18. Bowie CR, Medalia A. Bridging groups. In: Medalia A, Bowie CR, editors. Cognitive Remediation to Improve Functional Outcomes. New York, NY: Oxford (2016). p. 24–46.

Google Scholar

19. Keefe RS, Vinogradov S, Medalia A, Buckley PF, Caroff SN, D’Souza DC, et al. Feasibility and pilot efficacy results from the multisite cognitive remediation in the schizophrenia trials network (CRSTN) randomized controlled trial. J Clin Psychiatry (2012) 73(7):1016–22. doi:10.4088/JCP.11m07100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Bowie CR, McGurk SR, Mausbach B, Patterson TL, Harvey PD. Combined cognitive remediation and functional skills training for schizophrenia: effects on cognition, functional competence, and real-world behavior. Am J Psychiatry (2012) 169(7):710–8. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11091337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Strauss GP, Waltz JA, Gold JM. A review of reward processing and motivational impairment in schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull (2014) 40(Suppl 2):S107–16. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbt197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Wolf DH, Satterthwaite TD, Kantrowitz JJ, Katchmar N, Vandekar L, Elliott MA, et al. Amotivation in schizophrenia: integrated assessment with behavioral, clinical, and imaging measures. Schizophr Bull (2014) 40(6):1328–37. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbu026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Strassnig MT, Raykov T, O’Gorman C, Bowie CR, Sabbag S, Durand D, et al. Determinants of different aspects of everyday outcome in schizophrenia: the roles of negative symptoms, cognition, and functional capacity. Schizophr Res (2015) 165(1):76–82. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2015.03.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Robertson BR, Prestia D, Twamley EW, Patterson TL, Bowie CR, Harvey PD. Social competence versus negative symptoms as predictors of real world social functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2014) 160(1–3):136–41. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2014.10.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Kalin M, Kaplan S, Gould F, Pinkham AE, Penn DL, Harvey PD. Social cognition, social competence, negative symptoms and social outcomes: inter-relationships in people with schizophrenia. J Psychiatr Res (2015) 68:254–60. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Gold JM, Waltz JA, Frank MJ. Effort cost computation in schizophrenia: a commentary on the recent literature. Biol Psychiatry (2015) 78(11):747–53. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Fisher M, Loewy R, Carter C, Lee A, Ragland JD, Niendam T, et al. Neuroplasticity-based auditory training via laptop computer improves cognition in young individuals with recent onset schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull (2015) 41(1):250–8. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbt232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Loewy R, Fisher M, Schlosser DA, Biagianti B, Stuart B, Mathalon DH, et al. Intensive auditory cognitive training improves verbal memory in adolescents and young adults at clinical high risk for psychosis. Schizophr Bull (2016) 42(Suppl 1):S118–26. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbw009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Wood S, Sage JR, Shuman T, Anagnostaras SG. Psychostimulants and cognition: a continuum of behavioral and cognitive activation. Pharmacol Rev (2014) 66(1):193–221. doi:10.1124/pr.112.007054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Swerdlow NR, Tarasenko M, Bhakta SG, Talledo J, Alvarez AI, Hughes EL, et al. Amphetamine enhances gains in auditory discrimination training in adult schizophrenia patients. Schizophr Bull (2016) 43(4):873–80. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbw148

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Michalopoulou PG, Lewis SW, Drake RJ, Reichenberg A, Emsley R, Kalpakidou AK, et al. Modafinil combined with cognitive training: pharmacological augmentation of cognitive training in schizophrenia. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol (2015) 25(8):1178–89. doi:10.1016/j.euroneuro.2015.03.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Gilleen J, Michalopoulou PG, Reichenberg A, Drake R, Wykes T, Lewis SW, et al. Modafinil combined with cognitive training is associated with improved learning in healthy volunteers – a randomised controlled trial. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol (2014) 24(4):529–39. doi:10.1016/j.euroneuro.2014.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Popova P, Popov TG, Wienbruch C, Carolus AM, Miller GA, Rockstroh BS. Changing facial affect recognition in schizophrenia: effects of training on brain dynamics. Neuroimage Clin (2014) 6:156–65. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2014.08.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Fisher M, Mellon SH, Wolkowitz O, Vinogradov S. Neuroscience-informed auditory training in schizophrenia: a final report of the effects on cognition and serum brain-derived neurotrophic factor. Schizophr Res Cogn (2016) 3:1–7. doi:10.1016/j.scog.2015.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Rebola N, Srikumar BN, Mulle C. Activity-dependent synaptic plasticity of NMDA receptors. J Physiol (2010) 588(Pt 1):93–9. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.2009.179382

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Nabeshima T, Mouri A, Murai R, Noda Y. Animal model of schizophrenia: dysfunction of NMDA receptor-signaling in mice following withdrawal from repeated administration of phencyclidine. Ann N Y Acad Sci (2006) 1086:160–8. doi:10.1196/annals.1377.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Hashimoto K, Malchow B, Falkai P, Schmitt A. Glutamate modulators as potential therapeutic drugs in schizophrenia and affective disorders. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci (2013) 263(5):367–77. doi:10.1007/s00406-013-0399-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Ressler KJ, Rothbaum BO, Tannenbaum L, Anderson P, Graap K, Zimand E, et al. Cognitive enhancers as adjuncts to psychotherapy: use of D-cycloserine in phobic individuals to facilitate extinction of fear. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2004) 61(11):1136–44. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.61.11.1136

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Goff DC, Cather C, Gottlieb JD, Evins AE, Walsh J, Raeke L, et al. Once-weekly D-cycloserine effects on negative symptoms and cognition in schizophrenia: an exploratory study. Schizophr Res (2008) 106(2–3):320–7. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2008.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Otto MW, Kredlow MA, Smits JA, Hofmann SG, Tolin DF, de Kleine RA, et al. Enhancement of psychosocial treatment with D-cycloserine: models, moderators, and future directions. Biol Psychiatry (2016) 80(4):274–83. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Buchanan RW, Javitt DC, Marder SR, Schooler NR, Gold JM, McMahon RP, et al. The cognitive and negative symptoms in schizophrenia trial (CONSIST): the efficacy of glutamatergic agents for negative symptoms and cognitive impairments. Am J Psychiatry (2007) 164(10):1593–602. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.06081358

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Goff DC, Keefe R, Citrome L, Davy K, Krystal JH, Large C, et al. Lamotrigine as add-on therapy in schizophrenia: results of 2 placebo-controlled trials. J Clin Psychopharmacol (2007) 27(6):582–9. doi:10.1097/jcp.0b013e31815abf34

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Cain CK, McCue M, Bello I, Creedon T, Tang DI, Laska E, et al. D-cycloserine augmentation of cognitive remediation in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2014) 153(1–3):177–83. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2014.01.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. D’Souza DC, Radhakrishnan R, Perry E, Bhakta S, Singh NM, Yadav R, et al. Feasibility, safety, and efficacy of the combination of D-serine and computerized cognitive retraining in schizophrenia: an international collaborative pilot study. Neuropsychopharmacology (2013) 38(3):492–503. doi:10.1038/npp.2012.208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Cook JA, Leff HS, Blyler CR, Gold PB, Goldberg RW, Mueser KT, et al. Results of a multisite randomized trial of supported employment interventions for individuals with severe mental illness. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2005) 62(5):505–12. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.62.5.505

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Almerie MQ, Okba Al Marhi M, Jawoosh M, Alsabbagh M, Matar HE, Maayan N, et al. Social skills programmes for schizophrenia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev (2015) (6):CD009006. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD009006.pub2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Roberts DL, Combs DR, Willoughby M, Mintz J, Gibson C, Rupp B, et al. A randomized, controlled trial of social cognition and interaction training (SCIT) for outpatients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Br J Clin Psychol (2014) 53(3):281–98. doi:10.1111/bjc.12044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Penn DL, Roberts DL, Combs D, Sterne A. Best practices: the development of the social cognition and interaction training program for schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Psychiatr Serv (2007) 58(4):449–51. doi:10.1176/ps.2007.58.4.449

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Tan BL, Lee SA, Lee J. Social cognitive interventions for people with schizophrenia: a systematic review. Asian J Psychiatr (2016). doi:10.1016/j.ajp.2016.06.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Kurtz MM, Richardson CL. Social cognitive training for schizophrenia: a meta-analytic investigation of controlled research. Schizophr Bull (2012) 38(5):1092–104. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbr036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. LaCava PG, Rankin A, Mahlios E, Cook K, Simpson RL. A single case design evaluation of a software and tutor intervention addressing emotion recognition and social interaction in four boys with ASD. Autism (2010) 14(3):161–78. doi:10.1177/1362361310362085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. McGurk SR, Mueser KT, Feldman K, Wolfe R, Pascaris A. Cognitive training for supported employment: 2-3 year outcomes of a randomized controlled trial. Am J Psychiatry (2007) 164(3):437–41. doi:10.1176/ajp.2007.164.3.437

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. McGurk SR, Mueser KT, Xie H, Welsh J, Kaiser S, Drake RE, et al. Cognitive enhancement treatment for people with mental illness who do not respond to supported employment: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Psychiatry (2015) 172(9):852–61. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.14030374

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Patterson TL, Mausbach BT, McKibbin C, Goldman S, Bucardo J, Jeste DV. Functional adaptation skills training (FAST): a randomized trial of a psychosocial intervention for middle-aged and older patients with chronic psychotic disorders. Schizophr Res (2006) 86(1–3):291–9. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2006.05.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Lindenmayer JP, McGurk SR, Khan A, Kaushik S, Thanju A, Hoffman L, et al. Improving social cognition in schizophrenia: a pilot intervention combining computerized social cognition training with cognitive remediation. Schizophr Bull (2013) 39(3):507–17. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbs120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Turkington D, Sensky T, Scott J, Barnes TR, Nur U, Siddle R, et al. A randomized controlled trial of cognitive-behavior therapy for persistent symptoms in schizophrenia: a five-year follow-up. Schizophr Res (2008) 98(1–3):1–7. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2007.09.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Drake RJ, Day CJ, Picucci R, Warburton J, Larkin W, Husain N, et al. A naturalistic, randomized, controlled trial combining cognitive remediation with cognitive-behavioural therapy after first-episode non-affective psychosis. Psychol Med (2014) 44(9):1889–99. doi:10.1017/S0033291713002559

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Vinogradov S, Fisher M, Warm H, Holland C, Kirshner MA, Pollock BG. The cognitive cost of anticholinergic burden: decreased response to cognitive training in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry (2009) 166(9):1055–62. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09010017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. O’Reilly K, O’Connell P, Donohoe G, Coyle C, O’Sullivan D, Azvee Z, et al. Anticholinergic burden in schizophrenia and ability to benefit from psychosocial treatment programmes: a 3-year prospective cohort study. Psychol Med (2016) 46(15):3199–211. doi:10.1017/S0033291716002154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Sharma T, Reed C, Aasen I, Kumari V. Cognitive effects of adjunctive 24-weeks rivastigmine treatment to antipsychotics in schizophrenia: a randomized, placebo-controlled, double-blind investigation. Schizophr Res (2006) 85(1–3):73–83. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2006.03.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Buchanan RW, Conley RR, Dickinson D, Ball MP, Feldman S, Gold JM, et al. Galantamine for the treatment of cognitive impairments in people with schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry (2008) 165(1):82–9. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07050724

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Keefe RS, Malhotra AK, Meltzer HY, Kane JM, Buchanan RW, Murthy A, et al. Efficacy and safety of donepezil in patients with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder: significant placebo/practice effects in a 12-week, randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Neuropsychopharmacology (2008) 33(6):1217–28. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1301499

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Shekhar A, Potter WZ, Lightfoot J, Lienemann J, Dube S, Mallinckrodt C, et al. Selective muscarinic receptor agonist xanomeline as a novel treatment approach for schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry (2008) 165(8):1033–9. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.06091591

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Smith RC, Amiaz R, Si TM, Maayan L, Jin H, Boules S, et al. Varenicline effects on smoking, cognition, and psychiatric symptoms in schizophrenia: a double-blind randomized trial. PLoS One (2016) 11(1):e0143490. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0143490

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Walling D, Marder SR, Kane J, Fleischhacker WW, Keefe RS, Hosford DA, et al. Phase 2 trial of an alpha-7 nicotinic receptor agonist (TC-5619) in negative and cognitive symptoms of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull (2016) 42(2):335–43. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbv072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Haig G, Wang D, Othman AA, Zhao J. The alpha7 nicotinic agonist ABT-126 in the treatment of cognitive impairment associated with schizophrenia in nonsmokers: results from a randomized controlled phase 2b study. Neuropsychopharmacology (2016) 41(12):2893–902. doi:10.1038/npp.2016.101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Olincy A, Harris JG, Johnson LL, Pender V, Kongs S, Allensworth D, et al. Proof-of-concept trial of an alpha7 nicotinic agonist in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2006) 63(6):630–8. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.63.6.630

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Freedman R, Olincy A, Buchanan RW, Harris JG, Gold JM, Johnson L, et al. Initial phase 2 trial of a nicotinic agonist in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry (2008) 165(8):1040–7. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07071135

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Keefe RS, Meltzer HA, Dgetluck N, Gawryl M, Koenig G, Moebius HJ, et al. Randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study of encenicline, an alpha7 nicotinic acetylcholine receptor agonist, as a treatment for cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology (2015) 40(13):3053–60. doi:10.1038/npp.2015.176

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Zhang XY, Liu L, Liu S, Hong X, Chen DC, Xiu MH, et al. Short-term tropisetron treatment and cognitive and P50 auditory gating deficits in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry (2012) 169(9):974–81. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11081289

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Arnsten AF, Cai JX, Murphy BL, Goldman-Rakic PS. Dopamine D1 receptor mechanisms in the cognitive performance of young adult and aged monkeys. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (1994) 116(2):143–51. doi:10.1007/BF02245056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. George MS, Molnar CE, Grenesko EL, Anderson B, Mu Q, Johnson K, et al. A single 20 mg dose of dihydrexidine (DAR-0100), a full dopamine D1 agonist, is safe and tolerated in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2007) 93(1–3):42–50. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2007.03.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Rosell DR, Zaluda LC, McClure MM, Perez-Rodriguez MM, Strike KS, Barch DM, et al. Effects of the D1 dopamine receptor agonist dihydrexidine (DAR-0100A) on working memory in schizotypal personality disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology (2015) 40(2):446–53. doi:10.1038/npp.2014.192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. McClure MM, Harvey PD, Goodman M, Triebwasser J, New A, Koenigsberg HW, et al. Pergolide treatment of cognitive deficits associated with schizotypal personality disorder: continued evidence of the importance of the dopamine system in the schizophrenia spectrum. Neuropsychopharmacology (2010) 35(6):1356–62. doi:10.1038/npp.2010.5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Kelleher JP, Centorrino F, Huxley NA, Bates JA, Drake JK, Egli S, et al. Pilot randomized, controlled trial of pramipexole to augment antipsychotic treatment. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol (2012) 22(6):415–8. doi:10.1016/j.euroneuro.2011.10.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Arnsten AF, Cai JX, Goldman-Rakic PS. The alpha-2 adrenergic agonist guanfacine improves memory in aged monkeys without sedative or hypotensive side effects: evidence for alpha-2 receptor subtypes. J Neurosci (1988) 8(11):4287–98.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

77. Friedman JI, Adler DN, Temporini HD, Kemether E, Harvey PD, White L, et al. Guanfacine treatment of cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology (2001) 25(3):402–9. doi:10.1016/S0893-133X(01)00249-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. McClure MM, Barch DM, Romero MJ, Minzenberg MJ, Triebwasser J, Harvey PD, et al. The effects of guanfacine on context processing abnormalities in schizotypal personality disorder. Biol Psychiatry (2007) 61(10):1157–60. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.06.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Friedman JI, Carpenter D, Lu J, Fan J, Tang CY, White L, et al. A pilot study of adjunctive atomoxetine treatment to second-generation antipsychotics for cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. J Clin Psychopharmacol (2008) 28(1):59–63. doi:10.1097/jcp.0b013e318161318f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Kelly DL, Buchanan RW, Boggs DL, McMahon RP, Dickinson D, Nelson M, et al. A randomized double-blind trial of atomoxetine for cognitive impairments in 32 people with schizophrenia. J Clin Psychiatry (2009) 70(4):518–25. doi:10.4088/JCP.08m04358

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: schizophrenia, cognition, disability, everyday functioning, pharmacological cognitive enhancement, combination therapy

Citation: Harvey PD and Sand M (2017) Pharmacological Augmentation of Psychosocial and Remediation Training Efforts in Schizophrenia. Front. Psychiatry 8:177. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00177

Received: 28 June 2017; Accepted: 06 September 2017;
Published: 25 September 2017

Edited by:

Tomiki Sumiyoshi, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Japan

Reviewed by:

Chika Sumiyoshi, Fukushima University, Japan
Junghee Lee, University of California, Los Angeles, United States
Mahesh Menon, University of British Columbia, Canada

Copyright: © 2017 Harvey and Sand. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Philip D. Harvey, pharvey@miami.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.