Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 05 February 2018
Sec. Forensic Psychiatry

Validation of the Portuguese Version of Impulsive–Premeditated Aggression Scale in an Inmate Population

\r\nJacinto Costa Azevedo,*Jacinto Costa Azevedo1,2*Jos Luís Pais-RibeiroJosé Luís Pais-Ribeiro3Rui CoelhoRui Coelho1Margarida Figueiredo-Braga,\r\nMargarida Figueiredo-Braga1,2
  • 1Department of Neuroscience and Mental Health, Faculty of Medicine, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal
  • 2I3S – Instituto de Investigação e Inovação em Saúde, Porto, Portugal
  • 3Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, University of Porto, Porto, Portugal

Aggression is one of the core symptoms of antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) with therapeutic and prognostic relevance. ASPD is highly prevalent among inmates, being responsible for adverse events and elevated direct and indirect economic costs for the criminal justice system. The Impulsive/Premeditated Aggression Scale (IPAS) is a self-report instrument that characterizes aggression as either predominately impulsive or premeditated. This study aims to determine the validity and reliability of the IPAS in a sample of Portuguese inmates. A total of 240 inmates were included in the study. A principal component factor analysis was performed so as to obtain the construct validity of the IPAS impulsive aggression (IA) and premeditated aggression (PM) subscales; internal consistency was determined by Cronbach’s alpha coefficient; convergent and divergent validity of the subscales were determined analyzing correlations with the Barratt Impulsiveness scale, 11th version (BIS-11), and the Psychopathic Checklist Revised (PCL-R). The rotated matrix with two factors accounted for 49.9% of total variance. IA subscale had 11 items and PM subscale had 10 items. The IA and PM subscales had a good Cronbach’s alpha values of 0.89 and 0.88, respectively. The IA subscale is correlated with BIS-11 attentional, motor, and non-planning impulsiveness dimensions (p < 0.05). The PM subscale is correlated with BIS-11 attentional, motor impulsiveness dimensions (p < 0.05). The PM subscale is correlated with PCL-R interpersonal, lifestyle, and antisocial dimensions (p < 0.05). The IA subscale is not correlated with PCL-R. The Portuguese translated version of IPAS has adequate psychometric properties, allowing the measurement of impulsive and premeditated dimensions of aggression.

Introduction

Aggression represents a public health issue. Having a negative impact on individuals and society (1), it can be defined as a “behavioral display in which physical force is used with the intent to harm or damage another individual or object” (2).

A clinical definition of aggressive behavior should consider biological, environmental, cultural, and social variables, and their interplay to act as predisposition or eliciting factors (3). The range of behaviors that can be classified as aggressive can vary from verbal aggression to homicide, and it is important to characterize the level of planning, the possible understanding of hypothetical consequences, the presence of frustrations, insults, interpersonal attack, threats, environmental stressors, and associated psychopathologies.

Empirical literature has proposed a dichotomous aggression classification, i.e., impulsive aggression (IA; reactive, affective, or non-planned), and premeditated aggression (PM; proactive, instrumental, predatory, or controlled) (4). The expression “IA” refers to uncontrolled aggressive outbursts that are out of proportion to the provoking event, while “PM” describes aggressive behaviors that are planned, controlled, and/or goal-oriented (5).

According to the aggression type, individuals may differ in social adjustment, criminal behavior, emotional function, cognitive performance, autonomic response, and treatment outcome (6, 7).

The IA tends to occur in the presence of triggering stimulus that is interpreted as threat or provocation (8). This type of aggression involves affective arousal, leading to a rapid and uncontrolled behavioral response. It has been correlated with information-processing/neurocognitive impairments, abusive home backgrounds, an angry/impulsive/anxious personality, and high psychological stress reactivity (9).

The PM, by contrast, tends to be a planned behavior that has a specific expected goal (8), and it can be explained as a learned behavior (10). It has been correlated with poor parental control, lack of affect, psychopathic personality, and low physiological arousal (9).

The classification of an aggressive act or a pattern of aggressive behavior allows treatment selection and violence risk management. The IA has a good response to pharmacologic treatment when mood stabilizers and/or antipsychotics are prescribed (11). In the case of PM, on the contrary, the response to pharmacological treatment is generally insufficient (12), and forensic/behavioral strategies are recommended (7). Furthermore, individuals with PM have a higher risk of violent criminal recidivism than those with IA (13).

There is a link between aggressive behavior and psychiatric disorders (14).

Individuals with depression, anxiety, psychosis, substance use disorders, hyperactive attention deficit disorder, and personality disorders present a higher frequency of aggressive acts (15).

Impulsive aggression has been linked to psychiatric diseases, such as anxiety, depression, personality disorders, and substance use disorders (16). PM has been associated with psychopathic personality traits (17).

In particular, antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) is characterized by significant irritability, agitation, impulsiveness, and hostility, and aggression represents one of its core symptoms (18). ASPD is highly prevalent among inmates. It is, thus, responsible for adverse events and elevated direct and indirect economic costs in the criminal justice system (19).

Antisocial personality disorder is the only acknowledged psychiatric disorder that confers an increased risk for both IA and PM (2022), with several studies reporting psychopathy and psychopathic traits as risk factors for PM (23).

Psychopathic Checklist Revised (PCL-R) is the gold standard for psychopathy assessment and diagnosis, and separates psychopathic traits into four dimensions: (1) interpersonal, (2) affective, (3) lifestyle, and (4) antisocial (24).

Impulsivity has been related to aggression and is a symptom of ASPD (25). Impulsivity can be a necessary factor for aggression development (26). It can be defined “as a predisposition toward rapid, unplanned reactions to internal or external stimuli without regard to the negative consequences of these reactions to the impulsive individuals or to others” (27).

Impulsivity can be measured with Barratt Impulsiveness Scale, 11th version (BIS-11), a self-report psychometric instrument that separate impulsivity into three components: (1) acting on the spur of the moment (motor activation), (2) not focusing on the task at hand (attention), and (3) not planning and thinking carefully (lack of planning) (28). Individuals with scores higher than 72 are considered highly impulsive (29).

Patients with substance use disorders, depression, bipolar disorder, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (AD/HD), as well as suicide attempters and criminal offenders tend to have higher BIS-11 total scores (29).

Despite the evidence supporting the notion that persons who display an impulsive aggressive behavior are distinct from their counterparts who present a premeditated behavior, the clinical usefulness and generalization is questioned because most studies included violent incarcerated offenders, or psychiatric inpatients.

Within the incarcerated population, however, aggression is a relevant daily clinical concern. Therefore, the study of aggression in forensic psychiatric settings should be encouraged in order to find and promote the best clinical practices (30, 31).

It is important to use validated instruments in the evaluation of aggressive acts in prisoners in order to improve medical interventions (32). In Portugal, we do not have a validated aggression categorization instrument.

There are two psychometric instruments that allow aggression categorization: the impulsive–premeditated aggression scale (IPAS) and the reactive–proactive aggression questionnaire (2, 33, 34).

In the original validation study of IPAS, the sample comprised 93 physically aggressive men recruited in the community with a mean age of 35.9 years, whose principal aggression type was physical assault (n = 80). The aggression was measured through the Lifetime History of Aggression, Buss Perry Aggression Questionnaire, and the Aggression Interview. The principal component analysis identified two factors (IA and PM), which accounted for 16.56 and 14.03% of total variance, respectively. The internal consistency measured by Cronbach’s alpha was 0.77 for the IA factor and 0.82 for the PM factor. Sensitivity for the IA scale was 0.96 and specificity was 0.50. For the PM scale, sensitivity and specificity were 0.60 and 0.96, respectively (2).

All of the validation studies of IPAS reported identical results in principal component analysis, with two factors IA and PM. Internal consistency coefficients varied between 0.70 and 0.93 for IA and between 0.66 and 0.90 for PM (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Validation studies of Impulsive/Premeditated Aggression Scale (IPAS).

We intend to validate the Portuguese version of the IPAS in inmates and to determine its psychometric properties. Thus, we expect to replicate the purported two-factor model of IPAS; we predict that the subscales of the IPAS and the BIS-11 will be positively correlated; we predict that the PM subscales of the IPAS—unlike the IA ones—will be correlated with the PCL-R.

This study is a part of a larger study aiming to characterize biological and psychosocial factors that predict the aggression type in young adult male inmates.

Materials and Methods

Our sample was collected at two penitentiary institutions in the North of Portugal. The research protocol was formally approved by the Ethics Committee of Centro Hospitalar de São João, Porto, Portugal (Document number 48.14), and by the hosting institution, the General Direction of Probation and Prison Services. Participation was voluntary and there was no reward for it. The participants were all Portuguese. Written informed consent was obtained after the procedures were explained to the participants, in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Participants were included if they were over 18 years old, had a personal history of aggression in the past 6 months, had been referred to the clinical services for aggressions toward other inmates, were able to read, and if they were capable of providing their written informed consent. Socio-demographic characteristics and forensic history were collected from interviews and clinical records. We applied three psychometric instruments: the IPAS, the BIS-11, and the PCL-R. This sample does not intend to represent all the Portuguese inmate population, as the methods involve a non-probabilistic sample approach.

Transcultural Adaptation and Validation

In order to address the transcultural adaptation and validation of IPAS, an initial translation was carried out by a group of Portuguese experts. The back-translation of this first translated version was then performed by a native English-speaking expert. The first group analyzed the semantic and idiomatic correspondence of the translation and the back-translation, and performed a final synthesis translation (43).

Psychometric Instruments

Impulsive–Premeditated Aggression Scale

The IPAS is a 30-item self-report questionnaire used to classify aggressive acts occurring over the previous 6 months. Items are scored within a five-point Likert scale (1 = Strongly Disagree to 5 = Strongly Agree). The scale differentiates two factors: IA and PM, with the weighted score allowing for the categorization of the type of aggression. The individual’s level of IA and PM was obtained through the sum of 20 of the 30 items of the IPAS: the IA items (eight items: 3, 5, 7, 8, 9, 21, 24, and 26) and the PM items (12 items: 1, 2, 6, 10, 11, 12, 14, 16, 17, 20, 29, and 30) (2).

Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11

To determine the convergent validity of the IA and the PA subscales of the IPAS, we have utilized BIS-11. The BIS-11 is a self-report questionnaire for assessing general impulsiveness. The current scale version contains 30 items that are coded from 1 (rarely/never) to 4 (almost always/always). The level of impulsiveness is calculated by summing up the scores for each item. All items were defined as identifying impulsiveness within the structure of related personality traits. The factor analysis revealed three components as follows: “attentional impulsiveness,” “motor impulsiveness,” and “non-planning impulsiveness.” Instead of a unique total score, the study of the individual contribution of each component is recommended. Findings report an acceptable/high internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha 0.79–0.82) of the scale when applied to forensic samples (28). The structural properties of the BIS-11 were replicated in Portuguese speaking samples (44).

Psychopathy Checklist Revised

To determine the divergent validity of the IA and the PA subscales of the IPAS, we have applied the PCL-R. The PCL-R is the gold standard measure of psychopathy, gathering information from records and a semi-structured interview (24, 25). The 20 items are scored as absent (0), present to some degree (1), or fully present (2), having a maximum total score of 40 points. This instrument may be considered a four-factor model comprising interpersonal, affective, lifestyle, and antisocial dimensions (45). The interpersonal and affective dimensions jointly serve as second-order factors representing the core traits of the psychopathic personality. Lifestyle and antisocial facets form a superordinate factor of social deviance (46). The two second-order factors (Factor 1 and Factor 2) are concomitant with the original factor structure reported for the first edition of the PCL-R (47). The structural properties of the PCL-R were replicated in Portuguese samples for the standard protocol, including record review and structured interview (48). The PCL-R was applied by an experienced psychiatrist (Jacinto Costa Azevedo).

Statistical Analyses

Statistical analyses were conducted following the methodology used in the previous IPAS validation studies in adults. The principal component analysis was conducted with no assumptions regarding the number of potential factors. Lautenschlager’s (49) tables were used to determine the threshold for significant factors (minimum eigenvalue of 2.0) and item factor loadings (minimum eigenvalue of 0.40). In order to assess factor structure and maximize the reliability of factor interpretation, an Oblimin rotation was used so as to obtain an oblique factor solution of the original factors (50). Internal consistency was measured using Cronbach’s alpha (51). Convergent and divergent validity of the IPAS was tested by examining the Pearson’s product-moment correlations with the standardized measures of impulsivity and personality (BIS-11 and PCL-R). Confirmatory factorial analysis was tested by calculating goodness-of-fit indice, robust method (52).

The analyses were carried out using IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 22.0 (Armonk, NY, USA: IBM Corp.), and with the Structural Equation Program, EQS version 6.1 (53).

Results

Sample Description

The sample comprised 240 inmates with a mean (±SD) age of 35.4 ± 8.4 years; a total of 67.1% (n = 161) of the participants were single, and 58.3% did not have any children (n = 140). The mean education level of the sample was 6.8 ± 3.2 years. The mean time in prison (time spent behind bars at the time of assessment) of the sample was 109.3 ± 70.6 months. A total of 45.8% (n = 110) of the inmates had been convicted of violent crimes (physical assault, murder, attempted murder). Validation procedures were performed in the total sample (n = 240).

Principal Component Analysis

We have performed a principal component analysis exploring the original 30 items. First, we inspected the Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin Measure of Sampling Adequacy (KMO = 0.87) and reached the conclusion that it was an appropriate value. Second, we achieved a statistically significant Bartlett’s Test of Sphericity (p < 0.01). Third, we extracted two factors (with an oblimin rotation) with eigenvalues of 7.19 (Factor 1, IA) and 3.29 (Factor 2, PM) accounting for 34.27 and 15.65% of total variance, respectively. The IA factor was composed of ten items: 30, 27, 22, 9, 24, 15, 26, 4, 7, and 13. The PM factor was composed of eleven items: 6, 14, 29, 28, 2, 23, 12, 16, 20, 10, and 1. The items 3, 5, 8, 11, 17, 18, 19, 21, and 25 were excluded from the analysis as they exhibited component loadings inferior to 0.40 (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Factor loadings of Impulsive/Premeditated Aggression Scale (IPAS).

Confirmatory Factorial Analysis

To test the hypothesis of two dimensions, we use the confirmatory factor analysis, to test the goodness-of-fit indice, robust method. Results showed a comparative fit index of 0.90, and a root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) of 0.06, 90% confidence interval (0.05, 0.08).

Internal Consistency

The internal consistency test of the dimensions obtained in the factor analysis with the present sample revealed a good Cronbach’s alpha value for the IA factor (0.89), and a good Cronbach’s alpha value for the PM factor (0.88) (Table 2).

Reliability was also tested regarding the original version of the IPAS subscales. Cronbach’s alpha for the IA original subscale was acceptable (0.78), and it was good for the PM subscale (0.85).

Convergent and Divergent Validity

We have studied the convergence between the IPAS and the BIS-11. The statistically significant correlations between the two scales (IPAS and BIS-11) and between its dimensions are detailed in Table 3, showing correlations with attentional, motor, and non-planning dimensions (2, 5, 28). Correlations were statistically significant, suggesting the existence of convergence between the two scales.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Pearson correlations coefficients between Impulsive/Premeditated Aggression Scale (IPAS), Barratt Impulsiveness Scale, 11th version (BIS-11), and Psychopathic Checklist Revised (PCL-R).

We have studied divergent validity through the analysis of correlations between IPAS and PCL-R in a subsample of 134 inmates. The statistically significant correlations between the two scales (IPAS and PCL-R) and between its dimensions are detailed in Table 3. The IA subscale is not correlated with PCL-R. The PM subscale is correlated with PCL-R interpersonal, lifestyle, and antisocial dimensions (p < 0.05).

Discussion

According to AERA-APA-NCME (54), theoretical and empirical pieces of evidence that support the interpretations of test scores are fundamental so as to indicate the degree to which scores capture important aspects of the construct, as well as its relevance to the proposed use. The present results show appropriate validity for the Portuguese version of the IPAS.

The items of the instrument were adapted according to the original version (2). In our sample, IPAS was able to demonstrate the presence of two factors: impulsive and PM.

All the authors who studied the validity of IPAS were able to reduce the variability of the scale to two factors: IA and PM factors. This is in line with the literature on human aggression which argues that aggression is not a single construct and should be understood as being composed of two dimensions or categorized into two different types: impulsive or PM.

In the analysis of the explained variance described by other authors, we have observed that the IA factor can explain between 12.9% (41) and 24% (39) of the total IPAS variance; and that the PM factor can account for between 10.3% (42) and 15.6% (37) of the total IPAS variance. We have obtained higher values of explained variance (34.3% for the IA factor and 15.7% for the PM factor). In the analysis of the IPAS internal consistency described by other authors, we have observed that it varies between 0.70 (41) and 0.93 (40) for the IA factor; and between 0.66 (41) and 0.90 (40) for the PM factor. Our values of internal consistency and variance can be explained by the homogeneity of the sample.

According to Bentler and Douglas (52), when the goodness-of-fit and adjusted goodness-of-fit indexes are higher than 0.90, the analyses indicate adequate fit of the models. Also, according to Bentler and Douglas, when the RMSEA is less than. 0.10, the analysis indicates adequate fit of the models.

In our study, we have assessed the convergent validity through the analysis of Pearson product-moment correlations coefficients between IPAS and BIS-11. We had significant correlations between the IA subscale and the BIS-11 motor activation, attention, and non-planning dimensions. We had significant correlations between the PM subscale and the BIS-11 motor activation, attention dimensions, but not for non-planning dimension.

We have found a slightly higher correlation between the total score of BIS-11 and IA than with the PM subscale of IPAS. Our results were line with Mathias and colleagues who reported significant correlations between the IPAS subscales and BIS-11’s total score (correlations coefficients of 0.39 for AI and 0.26 for PM) (37).

However, Stanford and colleagues have reported higher significant correlations between PM and the total score of BIS-11 (correlation coefficients of 0.21 for AI and 0.38 for PM) (2).

Our data reveal significant correlations between the three dimensions of impulsivity measured by BIS-11and IA subscale of IPAS. Similar results were reported by Chen and colleagues that obtained significant values for correlations between IA and the three dimensions of BIS-11 (correlation coefficients of 0.15 for attentional impulsiveness, 0.21 for motor impulsiveness, and 0.12 for non-planning impulsiveness and AI) (41).

In the case of PM subscale of IPAS, we did not observe significant correlations with the non-planning dimension of BIS-11. The same was reported by Chen and colleagues, who only reported a significant correlation of 0.19 between PM and attentional impulsiveness (41).

We can explain these results in three possible ways. First, it is assumed that impulsivity is related aggression, not doing any discrimination between dimensions and type of aggression (33). These results are in accordance with the hypothesis that impulsivity is related and can be a predisposing factor for aggression (26). On the other hand, we should note that there are impulsive individuals who are not aggressive, and therefore, impulsivity is not the only factor necessary for the development of aggressiveness (55). Second, since our sample is composed of inmates, we can assume a high prevalence of individuals with substance use disorders and ASPD (56). In these types of pathology, there are a high expression of impulsivity (57, 58).

Thus, we can explain the correlations between subscales of IPAS and BIS-11 subscales because they are individuals with greater impulsivity conferred by underlying psychopathology. The third possible explanation, related to the non-planning dimension of impulsivity, is perhaps those impulsive individuals who maintain planning ability are those who can develop and learn premeditated aggressive behavior. These findings support the definition of PM in which individuals with PM plan their action and, thus, have fewer impairments in action planning impulsivity dimension (9). The non-planning dimension of impulsivity is related to working memory (59) and executive functions, namely with the subscale of strategic planning of the executive function index (60).

These results support the need for use of appropriated psychometric scales for the evaluation of aggression, in order to go beyond the assumption that individuals with higher levels of impulsivity are more likely to display externalizing behavior and thus having a greater probability of showing aggressive behavior.

This was the rationale for the use of BIS-11 for the evaluation of convergent validity with IPAS.

We have evaluated the divergent validity by analyzing correlations between AI and PM factors and PCL-R. As a result, we have obtained significant correlations for PM and PCL-R interpersonal, lifestyle, and antisocial dimensions, and not significant correlations for AI and PCL-R. Stanford and colleagues, in 2008, evaluated correlations between Psychopathic Personality Inventory (PPI) and the AI and PM factors, having obtained significantly higher values of PPI in the individuals categorized as premeditated aggressors, that is, having higher PM scores (38).

These data are in agreement with the literature on psychopathy and the type of aggressiveness externalized by individuals with higher expression of psychopathic personality traits. Psychopathy seems to be a risk factor for PM, mainly in forensic samples (23).

Regarding the generalization of results, some limitations should be considered. Although we have worked with a large sample, it is exclusively composed by men, therefore we do not know if aggressive women will behave in the same manner. The participants are subject to long sentences, making it difficult to generalize the results so as to include inmates with shorter sentences. Also, participation in this kind of research in a forensic facility context can change the way inmates respond to the IPAS. We must take into consideration that the legal circumstances of participants can modify the type of answers. This sample was obtained in a prison for convicted individuals, in Northern Portugal. We do not know whether the scale psychometric characteristics are the same in other forensic samples. In future research, it may be useful to consider tools that can externally quantify the individual acts of aggression, such as the Modified-Overt Aggression Scale.

Ethics Statement

The research protocol was formally approved by the Ethics Committee of Centro Hospitalar de São João, Porto, Portugal (Document number 48.14). Written informed consent was obtained after the procedures had been explained to participants, in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Author Contributions

JA and MF-B contributed to the conception of the study. The data analysis was carried out by JA and JP. All authors contributed to the interpretation of the data. JA and MF-B wrote the manuscript. All authors revised the content critically and approved the final version.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of the paper.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Letícia Malta, MD, and Nurse João Pinto for their collaboration in the acquisition of the data. We would like to thank the Portuguese General Directorate of Prison Services for authorizing the research.

Funding

The authors had no funding in the preparation of this manuscript.

References

1. Siever LJ. Neurobiology of aggression and violence. Am J Psychiatry (2008) 165(4):429–42. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07111774

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Stanford MS, Houston RJ, Mathias CW, Villemarette-Pittman NR, Helfritz LE, Conklin SM. Characterizing aggressive behavior. Assessment (2003) 10(2):183–90. doi:10.1177/1073191103010002009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Felthous AR, Lake SL, Rundle BK, Stanford MS. Pharmacotherapy of impulsive aggression: a quality comparison of controlled studies. Int J Law Psychiatry (2013) 36(3–4):258–63. doi:10.1016/j.ijlp.2013.04.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Vitiello B, Stoff DM. Subtypes of aggression and their relevance to child psychiatry. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry (1997) 36(3):307–15. doi:10.1097/00004583-199703000-00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Barratt ES, Stanford MS, Dowdy L, Liebman MJ, Kent TA. Impulsive and premeditated aggression: a factor analysis of self-reported acts. Psychiatry Res (1999) 86(2):163–73. doi:10.1016/S0165-1781(99)00024-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Meloy JR. Empirical basis and forensic application of affective and predatory violence. Aust N Z J Psychiatry (2006) 40(6–7):539–47. doi:10.1080/j.1440-1614.2006.01837.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Meyer JM, Cummings MA, Proctor G, Stahl SM. Psychopharmacology of persistent violence and aggression. Psychiatr Clin North Am (2016) 39(4):541–56. doi:10.1016/j.psc.2016.07.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Berkowitz L. Aggression: Its Causes, Consequences, and Control. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill (1993).

Google Scholar

9. Cima M, Raine A. Distinct characteristics of psychopathy relate to different subtypes of aggression. Pers Individ Dif (2009) 47(8):835–40. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.06.031

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Bandura A. Social Foundations of Thought and Action: A Social Cognitive Theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, Inc (1986).

Google Scholar

11. Lane SD, Kjome KL, Moeller FG. Neuropsychiatry of aggression. Neurol Clin (2011) 29(1):49–64, vii. doi:10.1016/j.ncl.2010.10.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Khalifa N, Duggan C, Stoffers J, Huband N, Völlm BA, Ferriter M, et al. Pharmacological interventions for antisocial personality disorder. Cochrane Database Syst Rev (2010) (8):CD007667. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD007667.pub2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Swogger MT, Walsh Z, Christie M, Priddy BM, Conner KR. Impulsive versus premeditated aggression in the prediction of violent criminal recidivism. Aggress Behav (2015) 41(4):346–52. doi:10.1002/ab.21549

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Fazel S, Gulati G, Linsell L. Schizophrenia and violence: systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS Med (2009) 2009(6):e1000120. doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1000120

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Rampling J, Furtado V, Winsper C, Marwaha S, Lucca G, Livanou M, et al. Non-pharmacological interventions for reducing aggression and violence in serious mental illness: a systematic review and narrative synthesis. Eur Psychiatry (2016) 34:17–28. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2016.01.2422

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Swogger MT, Walsh Z, Houston RJ, Cashman-Brown S, Conner KR. Psychopathy and axis I psychiatric disorders among criminal offenders: relationships to impulsive and premeditated aggression. Aggress Behav (2010) 36:45–53. doi:10.1002/ab.20330

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Woodworth M, Porter S. In cold blood: characteristics of criminal homicides as a function of psychopathy. J Abnorm Psychol (2002) 111(3):436–45. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.111.3.436

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press (2013).

Google Scholar

19. Moran P. The epidemiology of antisocial personality disorder. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol (1999) 34(5):231–42. doi:10.1007/s001270050138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Cornell DG, Warren J, Hawk G, Stafford E, Oram G, Pine D. Psychopathy in instrumental and reactive violent offenders. J Consult Clin Psychol (1996) 64(4):783–90. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.64.4.783

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Frick PJ, Cornell AH, Barry CT, Bodin SD, Dane HE. Callous-unemotional traits and conduct problems in the prediction of conduct problem severity, aggression, and self-report of delinquency. J Abnorm Child Psychol (2003) 31(4):457–70. doi:10.1023/A:1023899703866

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Walsh Z, Swogger MT, Kosson DS. Psychopathy and instrumental violence: facet level relationships. J Pers Disord (2009) 23(4):416–24. doi:10.1521/pedi.2009.23.4.416

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Blais J, Solodukhin E, Forth AE. A meta-analysis exploring the relationship between psychopathy and instrumental versus reactive violence. Crim Justice Behav (2014) 41(7):797–821. doi:10.1177/0093854813519629

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Hare RD. Manual for the Revised Psychopathy Checklist, 2nd ed. Toronto, ON: Multi-Health Systems (2003).

Google Scholar

25. Houston RJ, Stanford MS, Villemarette-Pittman NR, Conklin SM, Helfritz LE. Neurobiological correlates and clinical implications of aggressive subtypes. J Forensic Neuropsychol (2003) 3:67–87. doi:10.1300/J151v03n04_05

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Hentges RF, Shaw DS, Wang MT. Early childhood parenting and child impulsivity as precursors to aggression, substance use, and risky sexual behavior in adolescence and early adulthood. Dev Psychopathol (2017):1–15. doi:10.1017/S0954579417001596

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Moeller FG, Barratt ES, Dougherty DM, Schmitz JM, Swann AC. Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. Am J Psychiatry (2001) 158:1783–93. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.158.11.1783

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale. J Clin Psychol (1995) 51(6):768–74. doi:10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::AID-JCLP2270510607>3.0.CO;2-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Stanford MS, Mathias CW, Dougherty DM, Lake SL, Anderson NE, Patton JH. Fifty years of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale: an update and review. Pers Individ Dif (2009) 47(5):385–95. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2009.04.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. World Health Organization. World Report on Violence and Health. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization (2002).

Google Scholar

31. American Psychiatric Association. Psychiatric Services in Correctional Facilities. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Pub (2015). 567 p.

Google Scholar

32. Kendall T, Pilling S, Tyrer P, Duggan C, Burbeck R, Meader N, et al. Guidelines: borderline and antisocial personality disorders: summary of NICE guidance. BMJ (2009) 338(7689):293–5. doi:10.1136/bmj.b93

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Suris A, Lind L, Emmett G, Borman PD, Kashner M, Barratt ES. Measures of aggressive behavior: overview of clinical and research instruments. Aggress Violent Behav (2004) 9(2):165–227. doi:10.1016/S1359-1789(03)00012-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Raine A, Dodge K, Loeber R, Gatzke-Kopp L, Lynam D, Reynolds C, et al. The reactive–proactive aggression questionnaire: differential correlates of reactive and proactive aggression in adolescent boys. Aggress Behav (2006) 32(2):159–71. doi:10.1002/ab.20115

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Kockler TR, Stanford MS, Nelson CE, Meloy JR, Sanford K. Characterizing aggressive behavior in a forensic population. Am J Orthopsychiatry (2006) 76(1):80–5. doi:10.1037/0002-9432.76.1.80

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Conner KR, Houston RJ, Sworts LM, Meldrum S. Reliability of the Impulsive-Premeditated Aggression Scale (IPAS) in treated opiate-dependent individuals. Addict Behav (2007) 32(3):655–9. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.06.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Mathias CW, Stanford MS, Marsh DM, Frick PJ, Moeller FG, Swann AC, et al. Characterizing aggressive behavior with the Impulsive/Premeditated Aggression Scale among adolescents with conduct disorder. Psychiatry Res (2007) 151(3):231–42. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2006.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Stanford MS, Houston RJ, Baldridge RM. Comparison of impulsive and premeditated perpetrators of intimate partner violence. Behav Sci Law (2008) 26(6):709–22. doi:10.1002/bsl.808

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Haden SC, Scarpa A, Stanford MS. Validation of the Impulsive/Premeditated Aggression Scale in college students. J Aggress Maltreat Trauma (2008) 17(3):352–73. doi:10.1080/10926770802406783

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Kuyck WG, de Beurs E, Barendregt M, van den Brink W. Psychometric evaluation of the Dutch version of the Impulsive/Premeditated Aggression Scale (IPAS) in male and female prisoners. Int J Forensic Mental Health (2013) 12(3):172–9. doi:10.1080/14999013.2013.819396

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Chen FR, Yang Y, Qian M. Chinese version of Impulsive/Premeditated Aggression Scale: validation and its psychometric properties. J Aggress Maltreat Trauma (2013) 22(2):175–91. doi:10.1080/10926771.2013.741664

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Romans L, Fresán A, Sentíes H, Sarmiento E, Berlanga C, Robles-García R, et al. Validation of the Impulsive/Premeditated Aggression Scale in Mexican psychiatric patients. Nord J Psychiatry (2015) 69(5):397–402. doi:10.3109/08039488.2014.994033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Guillemin F, Bombardier C, Beaton D. Cross-cultural adaptation of health-related quality of life measures: literature review and proposed guidelines. J Clin Epidemiol (1993) 46(12):1417–32. doi:10.1016/0895-4356(93)90142-N

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. von Diemen L, Szobot CM, Kessler F, Pechansky F. Adaptation and construct validation of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS 11) to Brazilian Portuguese for use in adolescents. Rev Bras Psiquiatr (2007) 29(2):153–6. doi:10.1590/S1516-44462006005000020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Hare D. Psychopathy as a risk factor for violence. Psychiatr Q (1999) 70(3): 181–97. doi:10.1023/A:1022094925150

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Hare D, Neumann CS. The PCL-R assessment of psychopathy. In: Patrick CJ, editor. Handbook of Psychopathy New York: Guilford Press(2006). 58–88.

Google Scholar

47. Hare D, Neumann CS. Psychopathy as a clinical and empirical construct. Annu Rev Clin Psychol (2008) 4:217–46. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091452

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Gonçalves R. Psicopatia e processos adaptativos à prisão: da intervenção para a prevenção. Braga: Universidade do Minho (1999).

Google Scholar

49. Lautenschlager GJ. A comparison of alternatives to conducting Monte Carlo analyses for determining parallel analysis criteria. Multivariate Behav Res (1989) 24:365–95. doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr2403_6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Jackson JE. Oblimin rotation. In: Armitage P, Colton T, editors. Encyclopedia of Biostatistics. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd (2005). 6 p.

Google Scholar

51. Nunnally JC, Bernstein IH. Psychometric Theory. 3rd ed. New York: McGraw-Hill (1994).

Google Scholar

52. Bentler P, Douglas G. Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychol Bull (1980) 88:588–606. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.88.3.588

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Bentler PM. EQS 6.1 Structural Equations Program. Encino, CA: Multivariate Software (2004).

Google Scholar

54. American Educational Research Association, American Psychological Association, National Council on Measurement in Education. Standards for Educational and Psychological Testing. Washington, DC: American Educational Research Association (1999).

Google Scholar

55. Scarpazza C, Sellitto M, di Pellegrino G. Now or not-now? The influence of alexithymia on intertemporal decision-making. Brain Cogn (2017) 114:20–8. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2017.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Fazel S, Danesh J. Serious mental disorder in 23000 prisoners: a systematic review of 62 surveys. Lancet (2002) 359(9306):545–50. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(02)07740-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Kreek MJ, Nielsen DA, Butelman ER, LaForge KS. Genetic influences on impulsivity, risk taking, stress responsivity and vulnerability to drug abuse and addiction. Nat Neurosci (2005) 8(11):1450–7. doi:10.1038/nn1583

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Mann FD, Engelhardt L, Briley DA, Grotzinger AD, Patterson MW, Tackett JL, et al. Sensation seeking and impulsive traits as personality endophenotypes for antisocial behavior: evidence from two independent samples. Pers Individ Dif (2017) 105:30–9. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2016.09.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Whitney P, Jameson T, Hinson JM. Impulsiveness and executive control of working memory. Pers Individ Dif (2004) 37:417–28. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2003.09.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Spinella M. Self-rated executive function: development of the executive function index. Int J Neurosci (2005) 115:649–67. doi:10.1080/00207450590524304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: aggression, antisocial personality disorder, psychopathy, impulsivity, impulsive–premeditated aggression scale, Barratt Impulsiveness scale, 11th version, Psychopathic Checklist Revised

Citation: Azevedo JC, Pais-Ribeiro JL, Coelho R and Figueiredo-Braga M (2018) Validation of the Portuguese Version of Impulsive–Premeditated Aggression Scale in an Inmate Population. Front. Psychiatry 9:10. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00010

Received: 25 October 2017; Accepted: 15 January 2018;
Published: 05 February 2018

Edited by:

Katarina Howner, Karolinska Institutet (KI), Sweden

Reviewed by:

Giuseppe Sartori, Università degli Studi di Padova, Italy
Erich Flammer, ZfP Suedwuerttemberg, Germany

Copyright: © 2018 Azevedo, Pais-Ribeiro, Coelho and Figueiredo-Braga. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jacinto Costa Azevedo, jacintoazevedo@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.