Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 06 March 2018
Sec. Neuroimaging

Working Memory Modulates Glutamate Levels in the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex during 1H fMRS

  • Brain Imaging Research Division, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Neurosciences, Wayne State University School of Medicine, Detroit, MI, United States

Glutamate is involved in excitatory neurotransmission and metabolic processes related to brain function. Previous studies using proton functional magnetic resonance spectroscopy (1H fMRS) have demonstrated elevated cortical glutamate levels by 2–4% during visual and motor stimulation, relative to periods of no stimulation. Here, we extended this approach to working memory cognitive task performance, which has been consistently associated with dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) activation. Sixteen healthy adult volunteers completed a continuous visual fixation “rest” task followed by a letter 2-back working memory task during 1H fMRS acquisition of the left dlPFC, which encompassed Brodmann areas 45 and 46 over a 4.5-cm3 volume. Using a 100% automated fitting procedure integrated with LCModel, raw spectra were eddy current-, phase-, and shift-corrected prior to quantification resulting in a 32s temporal resolution or 8 averages per spectra. Task compliance was high (95 ± 11% correct) and the mean Cramer-Rao Lower Bound of glutamate was 6.9 ± 0.9%. Relative to continuous passive visual fixation, left dlPFC glutamate levels were significantly higher by 2.7% (0.32 mmol/kg wet weight) during letter 2-back performance. Elevated dlPFC glutamate levels reflect increased metabolic activity and excitatory neurotransmission driven by working memory-related cognitive demands. These results provide the first in vivo demonstration of elevated dlPFC glutamate levels during working memory.

Introduction

Glutamate is the most abundant neurotransmitter in the brain: ~10–12 mM (1). In addition to its role as the primary excitatory neurotransmitter, glutamate is also involved in the metabolic processes such as the tricarboxylic acid [TCA] cycle and is a neurochemical intermediate for other metabolites, such as GABA and glutamine (Figure 1) (2). Functional proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy (1H fMRS) facilitates exploration of in vivo glutamate dynamics in localized brain volumes in response to task-related stimulation (3). Seminal 1H fMRS research, conducted at 7 T, demonstrated visual stimulation increased glutamate levels by ~2–4% in the occipital lobe (4, 5). Subsequent 1H fMRS studies observed significant glutamate modulation throughout the brain: (1) occipital lobe during visual stimulation (610); (2) motor cortex during finger tapping (11); (3) anterior cingulate cortex during Stroop task performance (1214); (4) hippocampus during associative memory (15); and (5) elsewhere (1618). Extensive 13C MRS research demonstrates that glutamate involved in excitatory neurotransmission (glutamate–glutamine cycling) and neuronal oxidative metabolism of glucose exhibit a tight, nearly 1:1, stoichiometric relationship in rodents (1924) and humans (2530) [see review (2)]. Thus, task-induced glutamate modulation, as measured via 1H fMRS, is interpreted as in vivo biomarker of increased metabolic activity and excitatory neurotransmission. Indeed, numerous studies have demonstrated task-induced glutamate modulation is co-localized with blood oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) activation (5, 7, 9, 10, 31).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. A schematic representation of the glutamatergic tripartite synapse is depicted. Black arrows illustrate relationships between molecular species. ACA, acetyl-CoA; αKG, α-ketoglutarate; GLC, glucose; GLN, glutamine; GLU, glutamate; LAC, lactate; OAA, oxaloacetate; PYR, pyruvate.

Working memory involves the neuronal representation of stimuli over a brief delay period (32). Electrophysiology research in non-human primates indicate that persistent neuronal spiking through feed-forward excitatory microcircuits in cortical layer III in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) is associated with the maintenance of location information during spatial working memory tasks (32). Follow-up pharmacological challenge studies illustrated the central role of glutamate binding post-synaptic N-methyl-d-aspartate (NMDA) receptors during spatial working memory (33). To date, only one human study has investigated the dynamic in vivo neurochemistry of working memory processes (34). Using 1H fMRS at 3 T, Michels and colleagues found GABA levels were elevated, relative to baseline, during the first block of a letter Sternberg working memory task (34). Interestingly, GABA levels decreased with each successive repetition of the task, despite no decrement in performance, which illustrated the dynamic nature of neurochemistry during working memory processes (34). The focus of that study was investigation of GABAergic dynamics and the authors did not report isolated glutamate levels (34). Thus, dynamic glutamate modulation during working memory processes in humans remains unclear.

Here, we used 1H fMRS to investigate glutamate levels in the dlPFC during working memory task performance. The letter 2-back task is a well-established working memory paradigm reliably associated with dlPFC activation (35). Consistent with the literature, we hypothesized that letter 2-back task performance would be associated with elevated in vivo glutamate levels, relative to passive visual fixation, in the left dlPFC.

Materials and Methods

Participants

The local Institutional Review Board approved all study procedures, which were conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki (1964). Male and female right-hand dominant volunteers between 18 and 30 years old without self-reported MR contraindications, psychiatric conditions, and not currently taking psychoactive medications were recruited locally. Eligible subjects provided informed consent and were compensated for their time. Sixteen participants, nine males and seven females, completed self-report measures including medication history, demographic questionnaire and contact information, a comprehensive MRI safety screen, and the MRI scan, which was approximately 60min.

Experimental Tasks

Prior to the 1H fMRS scan, participants were verbally instructed how to perform each experimental task (Figure 2). Outside the MRI scanner, each participant practiced the letter 2-back task until deemed proficient by the experimenter. Inside the MRI scanner, participants completed the continuous passive visual fixation task, which consisted of a 2s “Rest” prompt, followed by static, continuous fixation-cross presentation for 238s. Next, participants completed the working memory task, which consisted of a 3Hz flashing grayscale checkerboard task for 208s followed by 7 blocks of alternating periods of passive visual fixation cross of 32s and letter 2-back of 64s. Each block of interleaved passive visual fixation was initiated on screen with a 2s “Rest” prompt prior to a static fixation cross for 30s totaling 32s. During each 2-back block, instruction of a 4s “2-back” prompt was followed by serial presentation of 20 capitalized letters out of which, 6 were randomly-assigned targets. Each letter was displayed for 500ms followed by 2,500ms of blank screen. Subjects indicated via button press with their right index finger, if the letter on the screen matched the letter presented two previously. For each task block, response accuracy was quantified as a percentage of correct responses out of 6 target letters. Participants were not provided feedback about response accuracy.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The experimental tasks are depicted. Left panel: the continuous passive visual fixation task consisted of instructions (“Rest”; 2s) and static fixation cross (centered on-screen; 238s). Right panel: the letter 2-back task consisted of two phases: variance minimization and alternating periods of letter 2-back interleaved with periods of passive visual fixation. The variance minimization phase consisted of a flashing grayscale checkerboard (centered on-screen; 3Hz) presented for 208s. Previous research in our laboratory demonstrated that the flashing checkerboard minimized glutamate variance better than alternative approaches (36). The working memory task consisted of seven interleaved repetitions of passive visual fixation [32s; 2s of instructions (“Rest”), 30s of static fixation cross centered on-screen] and letter 2-back [64s; 4s of instructions (“2-back”), 20 capitalized letters presented serially 3s/letter (60s total); each letter was presented on screen for 500ms followed by 2,500ms of blank screen].

Neuroimaging

All imaging was conducted on a 3T Siemens Verio system with a 32-channel receive-only volume head coil. All scans were completed in the morning between 9:00 and 11:30 a.m. The imaging protocol was identical for each subject. High resolution T1-weighted structural scans were collected using the 3D Magnetization Prepared Rapid Gradient Echo (MPRAGE) sequence with the following parameters: TR = 2.2s, TE = 3ms, TI = 799ms, flip angle = 13°, field-of-view (FOV) = 256mm × 256mm × 160mm, acquired matrix = 176 × 256 × 160, and pixel resolution 1mm × 1mm × 1mm. Prior to 1H fMRS acquisition, a region of the left dlPFC (25mm × 25mm × 25mm) larger than 1H fMRS voxel was shimmed to improve B0-field homogeneity [FASTESTMAP (37)]. 1H fMRS spectra were continuously acquired every 16s during each experimental task, which included 15 spectra for the continuous visual fixation and 55 spectra for the 2-back task. The acquisition parameters included the PRESS sequence with OVS and VAPOR for water suppression, TE = 23ms, TR = 4.0s, 4 averages per spectrum, bandwidth = 2kHz, 2,048 complex data points, and no apodization or zero-filling. A relatively short TE reduced the influence of J-evolution and diffusion, and the relatively long TR ensured minimal T1-weighted effect on the acquired signal. Fully relaxed water unsuppressed 1H fMRS spectra from the same voxel location and using a TR = 10s and 2 averages were acquired immediately after the continuous passive visual fixation and after the 2-back task, which was used for absolute quantification calculations.

Voxel Placement

1H fMRS spectra were acquired from the left dlPFC with a voxel dimension of 15mm × 20mm × 15mm or 4.5cm3, which encompassed Brodmann areas 45 and 46 (Figure 3). Voxel location, which was determined a priori and based on the results from a meta-analysis of 2-back fMRI studies (35), was determined on the MNI standard brain. From there, the automated voxel placement (AVP) method (38), which mapped the voxel location from template space to subject space, was used to prescribe 15 of the 16 participants’ voxel locations. AVP was not used for one subject due to experimenter error.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Orthonormal slices of voxel overlap across all subjects are depicted in a template brain. The voxel (15mm × 20mm × 15mm; 4.5cm3) was located in the left dlPFC and included Brodmann areas 45 and 46. 3D geometric voxel overlap is indicated by the red-to-white color gradient (white = complete voxel overlap across all subjects; orange-red = incomplete overlap across subjects). Voxel placement reliability across all subjects was excellent (89.9%).

Analysis Strategy

1H fMRS spectra were analyzed using LCModel, version 6.3, with a simulated basis set (39). The first spectrum acquired for the continuous passive visual fixation and letter 2-back tasks was not considered in analyses due to a potential partial T1 saturation effect (40). Post-processing and metabolite quantification steps were 100% automated. Eddy current effects were corrected using the unsuppressed water signal (41). T1-weighted structural images were B1-field corrected, the brain extracted, and segmented into partial volume maps of cerebrospinal fluid, gray and white matter using FreeSurfer and FSL tools (42, 43). Finally, tissue composition within the MRS voxel and appropriate correction factors, such as T1 and T2 relaxation, were used to calculate absolute glutamate levels (mmol/kg wet weight) (44). Consecutive phase- and shift-corrected spectra were averaged for the 2-back blocks, continuous and interleaved passive visual fixation conditions, which resulted in spectra with 8 averages and 32s temporal resolution. This resulted in bisecting the 64s 2-back blocks into a first- and second-half outcome measurement for each 2-back task block, which are denoted as “2-back-A” and “2-back-B,” respectively. Thus, each task phase included seven 32s outcome measurements. Rationale for this approach was twofold: (1) We used a classical block design, often employed in fMRI studies. Thus, to avoid signal-to-noise-ratio (SNR) and glutamate fit bias, we contrasted task phases using a common temporal resolution. (2) Exploratory analyses examined dynamic glutamate levels across task-repetitions, consistent with prior research (34).

Shapiro–Wilk test of normality and skewness/kurtosis statistics were used to evaluate variable distributions prior to outcome analyses. Whenever necessary, extreme values (≥1 SD from nearest value) were winsorized. The extreme value was replaced with the nearest value such that distributions were normalized with minimal influence on group mean. A priori hypotheses were evaluated via four one-way repeated measures analyses of variance (rmANOVA) that separately contrasted glutamate levels during 2-back-A and 2-back-B vs. interleaved passive visual fixation and continuous passive visual fixation. Power analyses conducted using G*Power (v3.0.10) indicated that 16 subjects were adequate to detect a small-to-moderate within-subject main effect (f ≥ 0.20) across 7 task repetitions at the recommended power (0.80) and α = 0.05 for autocorrelated measures (r = 0.70) (45, 46). Descriptive statistics were used to clarify significant main effects. In addition, four hypothesis-generating exploratory analyses were conducted. First, exploratory zero-order correlations were used to examine possible relationships between 2-back response accuracy and glutamate levels (uncorrected; p < 0.05). Second, one-way rmANOVAs evaluated possible temporal glutamate effects for each task phase (Bonferroni-corrected; p < 0.0125). Third, we evaluated other metabolites measured with Cramer Rao Lower Bound (CRLB%) < 20%, which included N-acetyl-aspartate (NAA), phosphocreatine plus creatine (PCr+Cr), glycerophosphocholine plus phosphocholine (GPC+PC), and myo-Inositol, to determine the neurochemical specificity of working memory task-related modulation. For each metabolite, two-way rmANOVAs evaluated 2-back-A and 2-back-B separately using a Bonferroni-correction (p < 0.00625). Fourth, LCModel fit characteristics, which included glutamate CRLB%, full-width half maximum (FWHM), and SNR, were evaluated separately for 2-back-A and 2-back-B using two-way rmANOVAs that were Bonferroni-corrected (p < 0.0083).

Voxel overlap was quantified using the “avp_overlap” script included in the AVP suite (38). Each subject’s voxel was coregistered to template space, and 3D geometric voxel overlap percentage reflecting placement accuracy and voxel overlap across all subjects reflecting reliability were calculated. Descriptive statistics are presented as mean ± 1 SD, unless otherwise noted. In all figures, error bars depict ± 1 SEM. Coefficient of variation percentage (CV%) was calculated to evaluate glutamate signal variability across time.

Results

Sample Characteristics

The sample consisted of 16 healthy college-educated participants not taking psychoactive medications. The mean age of the participants was 24 years old (± 3.4 years; range: 19–30 years), and the sample composition was 50% Caucasian or Asian and 56% male.

Voxel Overlap

The voxel placement using AVP (38) was highly accurate with a mean 3D geometric overlap percentage relative to the template voxel of 92.3 ± 4.7% and reliable across participants with a shared voxel overlap of 89.9% (Figure 3). Mean voxel tissue composition was 36.8 ± 3.8% gray matter and 60.8 ± 4.5% white matter. The manual voxel placement of one participant’s voxel was less accurate to the template (77.7%) than AVP placed voxels (86.2–96.9%).

Behavioral Data

Behavioral data demonstrated high task compliance across subjects and task blocks (mean correct: 94.8 ± 10.7%; mean response latency: 644 ± 171ms). One-way rmANOVA indicated a significant increase in mean accuracy across blocks [Time effect; F(6,90) = 2.39, p < 0.05, from 88.5% in Block 1 to 96.9% in Block 7; Figure 4; upper panel]. One-way rmANOVA indicated mean response latency non-significantly decreased across blocks [F(6,90) = 1.90, p = 0.09, from 697ms in Block 1 to 651ms in Block 7; Figure 4; lower panel].

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Letter 2-back behavioral data are depicted. Upper panel: letter 2-back response accuracy (mean percentage correct ± 1 SEM) is depicted across task blocks. Response accuracy significantly improved across task repetitions (Time effect). Lower panel: letter 2-back response latency [mean response latency (ms) ± 1 SEM] is depicted across task blocks.

LCModel Fit Characteristics

LCModel fit characteristics are displayed in Table 1, and a representative spectrum is depicted in Figure 5. Exploratory analyses evaluated possible changes in LCModel fit characteristics as a function of task phase where 2-back-A and 2-back-B were considered separately. Two-way rmANOVAs indicated mean CRLB% of glutamate, FWHM, and SNR values did not differ across task blocks between 2-back and passive visual fixation (Table 1). These results indicate a non-significant BOLD effect on FWHM, and thus, linewidth broadening was not applied for subsequent analyses.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. LCModel Fit Characteristics by Task Phase.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. A representative 1H magnetic resonance spectroscopy spectrum is depicted (32s temporal resolution; 8 averages). The raw signal is depicted in black, while the LCModel fit is in red. The isolated glutamate (GLU) signal (blue line) and residual are presented below the spectrum. Chemical shift (ppm) is depicted below the spectrum. NAA, N-acetyl-aspartate; PCr+Cr phosphocreatine plus creatine; GPC+PC, glycerophosphocholine plus phosphocholine; myo-Ins, myo-Inositol.

2-Back Glutamate Modulation

As described above, glutamate levels during each 64s 2-back block were bisected into the first 32s, 2-back-A, and final 32s, 2-back-B, to match the 32s visual fixation measurements, such that the results were not biased by SNR and CRLB% of the glutamate fit. Overall, one-way rmANOVA indicated 2-back-A glutamate levels were significantly higher by 2.7% (0.32 mmol/kg wet wt.) than the continuous visual fixation levels [F(1,111) = 6.26, p = 0.014, partial η2 = 0.05; small-to-moderate effect size; 12.07 ± 0.85 vs. 11.75 ± 1.00 mmol/kg wet wt., respectively; Figure 6]. In contrast, 2-back-A glutamate levels did not differ from interleaved visual fixation levels (p = 0.92; 12.07 ± 0.85 vs. 12.06 ± 1.04 mmol/kg wet wt., respectively).

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Absolute glutamate levels (mmol/kg wet wt. ± 1 SEM) for each task phase are depicted. Mean 2-back-A glutamate levels (red) were significantly higher (2.7%; 0.32 mmol/kg wet wt.) than continuous passive visual fixation glutamate levels (gray). Mean 2-back-B glutamate levels (blue) were non-significantly higher (2.1%; 0.25 mmol/kg wet wt.; p = 0.07) than continuous passive visual fixation glutamate levels (gray). Mean interleaved visual fixation glutamate levels (white) did not differ from 2-back levels.

One-way rmANOVA indicated 2-back-B glutamate levels were non-significantly higher by 2.1% (0.25 mmol/kg wet wt.) than the continuous visual fixation levels [F(1,111) = 3.28, p = 0.07, partial η2 = 0.03; small-to-moderate effect size; 12.00 ± 0.98 vs. 11.75 ± 1.00 mmol/kg wet wt., respectively; Figure 6]. 2-back-B glutamate levels did not differ from interleaved visual fixation levels (p = 0.61; 12.00 ± 0.98 vs. 12.06 ± 1.04 mmol/kg wet wt., respectively).

Dynamic Glutamate Levels

Exploratory one-way rmANOVAs indicated that glutamate levels did not change across task repetitions for either 2-back-A or 2-back-B (p > 0.35). However, glutamate levels significantly increased across task repetitions during the interleaved passive visual fixation [F(6,90) = 5.54, p < 0.001, partial η2 = 0.27; large effect size; not shown]. The temporal dynamics of 2-back glutamate levels relative to mean continuous visual fixation levels are depicted in Figure 7.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Glutamate levels (±1 SEM) are depicted across task blocks. Glutamate levels during 2-back-A (red) and 2-back-B (blue) are depicted as a percentage relative to the mean continuous visual fixation levels.

Neurochemical Specificity

Metabolites other than glutamate were examined as a function of task demands: myo-Inositol, GPC+PC, PCr+Cr, and NAA. Exploratory two-way rmANOVAs indicated that metabolites (other than glutamate) did not significantly differ as a function of task phase relative to the continuous passive visual fixation.

Exploratory Bivariate Correlations

Exploratory analyses examined bivariate correlations between glutamate levels and response accuracy. No significant zero-order correlations were observed (p > 0.15).

Discussion

The goal of the present study was to detect changes in glutamate levels during performance of a well-established working memory task. Our findings indicated, for the first time in humans, that working memory processes increased in vivo glutamate levels in the dlPFC relative to the continuous passive visual fixation control condition. Throughout the remainder of the manuscript, we will interpret these findings, discuss study limitations, and describe future applications.

In the present study, 1H MRS spectra were continuously acquired from a small voxel with a volume of 4.5cm3 placed in the left dlPFC encompassing Brodmann areas 45 and 46 and analyzed at 32s temporal resolution with 8 averages per spectra. LCModel fit characteristics were used to confirm spectra quality. LCModel fit quality was consistent with previous 1H fMRS studies (31) and did not differ as a function task phase. CRLB% reflects the lower bound of the fitted parameters, and thus, it was critical to demonstrate that the quantification of glutamate was not biased by task phase (47). Relative to continuous passive visual fixation, in vivo left dlPFC glutamate levels were significantly higher by 2.7% (0.32mmol/kg wet wt.) during the first 32s and non-significantly higher by 2.1% (0.25mmol/kg wet wt.) during the final 32s of 2-back working memory task performance. The task related changes were specific to only glutamate, as changes in NAA, PCr+Cr, GPC+PC, and myo-Inositol were all non-significant. The magnitude of task-evoked glutamate modulation observed herein was consistent with prior 1H fMRS research (47, 10, 11). Finally, we found no significant bivariate correlations between response accuracy and glutamate levels, possibly due to a ceiling effect with mean response accuracy ~95%.

1H fMRS is a neuroimaging technique that facilitates direct measurement of dynamic neurochemistry levels. Seminal 1H fMRS research was conducted at high field, but technological advances have made detection of glutamate modulation possible at 3T (9, 15, 36). The 1H fMRS signal does not differentiate cell type, neuron vs. glia, or compartment, intracellular vs. extracellular, but is directly proportional to the concentration of molecules within the voxel. Thus, higher glutamate levels must reflect net formation of glutamate molecules. The putative neurobiological mechanism of net glutamate formation was clarified by extensive 13C MRS research which found a tight coupling between glutamate–glutamine neurotransmitter cycling rate and glucose metabolic rate (CMRGLC) in rodents across brain activity levels from isoelectricity to awake and supra-“basal” levels during stimulation (2, 20, 22, 23, 4850). Thus, extant literature indicates that stimulation-induced increases in glutamate levels, as measured by 1H fMRS, reflect increased excitatory neurotransmission and oxidative metabolism. We suggest that working memory-related demands involving neural maintenance of letters during the delay periods drove an increase in excitatory neural activity in the dlPFC, which necessitated an increase in glucose utilization and oxidative metabolism. Behavioral data confirmed that all participants were actively engaged in the 2-back task and demonstrated high working memory proficiency: ~95% correct. Based on well-controlled preclinical work (50), the putative neurobiological pathways associated with net glutamate formation during working memory-related demand are illustrated in Figure 8. Prior studies have proposed increased enzymatic flux through several pathways, including pyruvate carboxylase and glutamate dehydrogenase (11, 21, 50) or the malate-aspartate shuttle (5, 51), but commentary about specific pathways is beyond the scope of our data. Multimodal studies indicate that stimulation-evoked glutamate modulation corresponded with other modalities indicating neural spiking activity, including BOLD activation (9, 10, 31) and gamma-band activity (8).

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. A schematic representation of the glutamatergic tripartite synapse is depicted. Black arrows illustrate relationships between molecular species. Red arrows illustrate enzymatic or metabolic reactions that are hypothesized to be upregulated during neural stimulation or cognitive task performance based on well-controlled preclinical studies which observed an increase in glutamate-glutamine cycling [e.g., Ref. (50)]. Net formation of glutamate during stimulation (i.e., as observed in 1H fMRS studies) is hypothesized to be associated with elevated glucose (GLC) extraction and metabolism (CMRGLC) (50). ACA, acetyl-CoA; αKG, α-ketoglutarate; CMRGLC, cerebral metabolic rate of glucose; GLC, glucose; GLN, glutamine; VGS, glutamine synthetase rate; GLU, glutamate; LAC, lactate; OAA, oxaloacetate; PYR, pyruvate; VPC, pyruvate carboxylation rate; VTCA, TCA cycle rate.

One prior 1H fMRS study investigated dynamic neurochemistry during working memory (34). Michels and colleagues (34) found GABA levels were significantly higher during the initial working memory task block. They interpreted the significant GABA modulation as indicative of increased metabolic activity and GABA-glutamate–glutamine cycling during working memory, as we do here (34). Interestingly, they found a significant decrease in GABA levels and response latency, but no change in response accuracy, across successive working memory task block repetitions (34). They interpreted these data to reflect improved working memory proficiency with repeated task performance. We found a similar learning, but different neurochemical, effect. Response latency non-significantly decreased and response accuracy significantly increased, across task repetitions. However, 2-back-A and 2-back-B glutamate levels were unchanged across task blocks (Figure 7). Thus, participants demonstrated enhanced working memory proficiency, but comparable glutamatergic response across task repetitions. There was some evidence of a more robust glutamatergic response early, as opposed to later, within each 64s 2-back block with nominally greater modulation during 2-back-A > 2-back-B, which may indicate the novelty of task onset contributed to the signal.

The 1H fMRS glutamate signal is a biomarker of the relative levels of excitatory neural activity between two states: stimulation vs. “basal” levels. Thus, isolation of task-evoked modulation necessitates accurate measurement of both states, which can be challenging, especially in the dlPFC. We selected the continuous visual fixation cross as our “non-task-active” behavioral paradigm based on prior research (36). In that study, we found glutamate levels exhibited less variability and lower levels, thus reflecting a better approximation of the “basal” state than other behavioral paradigms tested, such as eyes closed, flashing checkerboard and finger tapping (36). This is consistent with results herein that glutamate levels significantly increased across time during the interleaved passive visual fixation, reflecting less constrained behavior and a poor measurement of the “basal” state, while 2-back glutamate levels were unchanged across task blocks. This complicated our isolation of working memory-evoked glutamate modulation and illustrates the challenge of measuring dynamic neurochemistry in prefrontal brain regions. Prior 1H fMRS studies, conducted in motor and sensory cortices, demonstrated compelling stimulation-evoked change in glutamate levels in part because measurement of “basal” glutamate levels is simplified in those brain regions (47, 10, 11, 31). In the occipital lobe, the absence of visual stimulation is an ideal comparison for periods of visual stimulation (4). In contrast, the dlPFC is involved in numerous cognitive processes (52, 53), including fundamental processes, such as attentional control (54, 55). In theory, any cognitive process sub-served by the dlPFC could drive an increase in phasic neural activity and thus modulate glutamate levels. Indeed, Michels and colleagues found that dlPFC GABA levels exhibited high variability (non-significant linear regression) across time during four consecutive “rest” periods (34). This subtle complexity may have contributed to different analytic strategies and presentation styles. 1H fMRS studies that investigated cognitive task-related glutamate modulation often analyzed glutamate levels relative to a single baseline measurement (13, 15, 34), while 1H fMRS studies in sensory and motor regions typically depict dynamic glutamate levels across time using classical block designs as was used herein (47, 10, 11, 31). Future 1H fMRS studies that investigate neurochemical response during cognition should select their “non-task-active” behavioral paradigms with care, as accurate measurement of “basal” glutamate levels for comparison is essential to avoid Type II error (36).

Cognitive task-based 1H fMRS, as a neuroimaging approach, has numerous potential future applications and advantages over more widely used techniques (e.g., BOLD fMRI) (3). First, 1H fMRS facilitates direct in vivo measurement of dynamic neurochemistry, including the principal excitatory and inhibitory neurotransmitters (glutamate and GABA, respectively). Second, 1H fMRS is not susceptible to the neurovascular coupling effects associated with fMRI studies (3). As such, future 1H fMRS studies could examine populations not well-suited for fMRI studies such as elderly subjects, individuals with vascular diseases, or taking vasoactive medications. Third, the neurochemical specificity of this approach affords advantages for pharmacological challenge studies or investigation of neurobiological mechanisms. In particular, 1H fMRS is well-positioned for investigation of cortical excitability and the balance of excitatory and inhibitory drive, which is central to cognition and has been implicated in numerous psychiatric diagnoses (56).

Alternative Explanations and Limitations

There are several alternative explanations for the present results. First, it is possible that T2 relaxation of glutamate changed during 2-back task performance. The equation S=S0eTE/T2 describes the relationship between the initial signal (S0), the measured signal (S), TE, and T2 relaxation (57). However, in order to explain the observed increase in glutamate concentration during 2-back, the T2 relaxation of glutamate would need to increase ~20% assuming glutamate has T2 value of 124ms at 3T in the dlPFC (58). Second, Type I error is possible. A small sample and winsorized distributions may increase likelihood of Type I error. However, results reported herein were hypothesized and consistent with published studies. In addition, the sample was unmedicated and healthy. Finally, we observed a small-to-moderate effect size using a rigorous experimental design.

This study has several limitations. First, as is the case with all single voxel MRS studies, this study was susceptible to partial volume effects. Three factors minimized the influence of partial volume effects on these findings: (1) voxel placement across subjects was highly reliable, (2) voxel tissue composition variability was low across subjects with gray and white matter CV% = 10.3 and 7.4%, respectively, and considered in the quantification of absolute glutamate concentration, and (3) only within-subject analyses across conditions were considered. A second limitation was our inability to reliably quantify other relevant molecules such as GABA, glutamine, aspartate, or lactate. A third limitation was voxel placement based on a BOLD fMRI meta-analysis (35) and not from the measured BOLD activation of the individual subjects. Fourth, we did not measure neurochemistry from a “control” voxel location. Finally, it is important to note that other 1H fMRS studies observed significant modulation of metabolites other than glutamate (17). As such, we chose not to report glutamate levels as a ratio relative to other metabolites such as PCr+Cr or NAA, because any modulation of those metabolites in time would confound interpretation. Indeed, prior 1H fMRS studies found significant NAA modulation as a function of task stimulation (5961) and others found non-significant, but notable, PCr+Cr modulation (10, 11).

Conclusion

The findings presented herein demonstrated, for the first time in humans, that working memory task performance modulated in vivo dlPFC glutamate levels in healthy adult volunteers. Results from this study were consistent with a priori hypotheses and prior research across modalities including fMRI, 1H fMRS, and electrophysiology. We interpret elevated glutamate levels during working memory task performance to reflect increased metabolic activity and excitatory neurotransmission driven by working memory-related demands. Future research studies are needed to replicate these effects and investigate possible “dose-response” relationships with glutamate modulation as a function of working memory cognitive load.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of Wayne State University Institutional Review Board with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the Wayne State University Institutional Review Board.

Author Contributions

EW authored the manuscript and developed the figures. EW and JS developed the experimental task, analyzed the data, and edited the manuscript. CA assisted with data analyses and edited the manuscript. DK operated the MRI scanner and assisted with data collection. VD assisted with experimental task development and edited the manuscript. All authors have read and approved of this manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

All authors declare no conflict of interest with respect to the conduct or content of this work.

Acknowledgments

Research reported in this publication was supported by the National Institute on Drug Abuse of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number F31 DA040369 (awarded to EW) and by the National Institute of Mental Health under Award Number R01 MH111177 (JAS and VAD). The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. Funding also provided by the State of Michigan (Joe Young Sr./Helene Lycaki funds; JS) and the Detroit Wayne Mental Health Authority (JS). Funding sources were not involved in the design, execution, analysis, or interpretation of data described in this manuscript. The authors thank Caroline Zajac-Benitez, Muzamil Arshad, and Jonathan Lynn for their assistance.

References

1. Erecińska M, Silver IA. Metabolism and role of glutamate in mammalian brain. Prog Neurobiol (1990) 35:245–96. doi:10.1016/0301-0082(90)90013-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Rothman DL, De Feyter HM, Graaf RA, Mason GF, Behar KL. 13C MRS studies of neuroenergetics and neurotransmitter cycling in humans. NMR Biomed (2011) 24:943–57. doi:10.1002/nbm.1772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Stanley JA, Raz N. Functional magnetic resonance spectroscopy: the “new” MRS for cognitive neuroscience and psychiatry research. Front Psychiatry (2018).

Google Scholar

4. Mangia S, Tkáč I, Gruetter R, Van De Moortele P-F, Giove F, Maraviglia B, et al. Sensitivity of single-voxel 1H-MRS in investigating the metabolism of the activated human visual cortex at 7 T. Magn Reson Imaging (2006) 24:343–8. doi:10.1016/j.mri.2005.12.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Mangia S, Tkáč I, Gruetter R, Van De Moortele P-F, Maraviglia B, Uğurbil K. Sustained neuronal activation raises oxidative metabolism to a new steady-state level: evidence from 1H NMR spectroscopy in the human visual cortex. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab (2007) 27:1055–63. doi:10.1038/sj.jcbfm.9600401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Lin Y, Stephenson MC, Xin L, Napolitano A, Morris PG. Investigating the metabolic changes due to visual stimulation using functional proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy at 7 T. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab (2012) 32:1484–95. doi:10.1038/jcbfm.2012.33

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Schaller B, Mekle R, Xin L, Kunz N, Gruetter R. Net increase of lactate and glutamate concentration in activated human visual cortex detected with magnetic resonance spectroscopy at 7 Tesla. J Neurosci Res (2013) 91:1076–83. doi:10.1002/jnr.23194

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Lally N, Mullins PG, Roberts MV, Price D, Gruber T, Haenschel C. Glutamatergic correlates of gamma-band oscillatory activity during cognition: a concurrent ER-MRS and EEG study. Neuroimage (2014) 85:823–33. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.07.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Apšvalka D, Gadie A, Clemence M, Mullins PG. Event-related dynamics of glutamate and BOLD effects measured using functional magnetic resonance spectroscopy (fMRS) at 3T in a repetition suppression paradigm. Neuroimage (2015) 118:292–300. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.06.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Bednařík P, Tkáč I, Giove F, Dinuzzo M, Deelchand DK, Emir UE, et al. Neurochemical and BOLD responses during neuronal activation measured in the human visual cortex at 7 Tesla. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab (2015) 35:601–10. doi:10.1038/jcbfm.2014.233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Schaller B, Xin L, O’Brien K, Magill AW, Gruetter R. Are glutamate and lactate increases ubiquitous to physiological activation? A 1H functional MR spectroscopy study during motor activation in human brain at 7 Tesla. Neuroimage (2014) 93:138–45. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.02.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Taylor R, Neufeld RW, Schaefer B, Densmore M, Rajakumar N, Osuch EA, et al. Functional magnetic resonance spectroscopy of glutamate in schizophrenia and major depressive disorder: anterior cingulate activity during a color-word Stroop task. NPJ Schizophr (2015) 1:15028. doi:10.1038/npjschz.2015.28

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Taylor R, Schaefer B, Densmore M, Neufeld RW, Rajakumar N, Williamson PC, et al. Increased glutamate levels observed upon functional activation in the anterior cingulate cortex using the Stroop task and functional spectroscopy. Neuroreport (2015) 26:107. doi:10.1097/WNR.0000000000000309

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Kühn S, Schubert F, Mekle R, Wenger E, Ittermann B, Lindenberger U, et al. Neurotransmitter changes during interference task in anterior cingulate cortex: evidence from fMRI-guided functional MRS at 3 T. Brain Struct Funct (2016) 221:2541–51. doi:10.1007/s00429-015-1057-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Stanley JA, Burgess A, Khatib D, Ramaseshan K, Arshad M, Wu H, et al. Functional dynamics of hippocampal glutamate during associative learning assessed with in vivo 1H functional magnetic resonance spectroscopy. Neuroimage (2017) 153:189–97. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.03.051

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Gussew A, Rzanny R, Erdtel M, Scholle HC, Kaiser WA, Mentzel HJ, et al. Time-resolved functional 1H MR spectroscopic detection of glutamate concentration changes in the brain during acute heat pain stimulation. Neuroimage (2010) 49:1895–902. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Gutzeit A, Meier D, Meier M, Von Weymarn C, Ettlin D, Graf N, et al. Insula-specific responses induced by dental pain. A proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy study. Eur Radiol (2011) 21:807–15. doi:10.1007/s00330-010-1971-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Huang Z, Davis HH IV, Yue Q, Wiebking C, Duncan NW, Zhang J, et al. Increase in glutamate/glutamine concentration in the medial prefrontal cortex during mental imagery: a combined functional MRS and fMRI study. Hum Brain Mapp (2015) 36:3204–12. doi:10.1002/hbm.22841

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Choi I-Y, Lei H, Gruetter R. Effect of deep pentobarbital anesthesia on neurotransmitter metabolism in vivo: on the correlation of total glucose consumption with glutamatergic action. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab (2002) 22:1343–51. doi:10.1097/00004647-200211000-00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. De Graaf RA, Mason GF, Patel AB, Rothman DL, Behar KL. Regional glucose metabolism and glutamatergic neurotransmission in rat brain in vivo. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2004) 101:12700–5. doi:10.1073/pnas.0405065101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Öz G, Berkich DA, Henry P-G, Xu Y, Lanoue K, Hutson SM, et al. Neuroglial metabolism in the awake rat brain: CO2 fixation increases with brain activity. J Neurosci (2004) 24:11273–9. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3564-04.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Patel AB, De Graaf RA, Mason GF, Rothman DL, Shulman RG, Behar KL. The contribution of GABA to glutamate/glutamine cycling and energy metabolism in the rat cortex in vivo. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2005) 102:5588–93. doi:10.1073/pnas.0501703102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Chowdhury GM, Patel AB, Mason GF, Rothman DL, Behar KL. Glutamatergic and GABAergic neurotransmitter cycling and energy metabolism in rat cerebral cortex during postnatal development. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab (2007) 27:1895–907. doi:10.1038/sj.jcbfm.9600490

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Van Eijsden P, Behar KL, Mason GF, Braun KP, De Graaf RA. In vivo neurochemical profiling of rat brain by 1H-[13C] NMR spectroscopy: cerebral energetics and glutamatergic/GABAergic neurotransmission. J Neurochem (2010) 112:24–33. doi:10.1111/j.1471-4159.2009.06428.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Gruetter R, Seaquist ER, Kim S, Ugurbil K. Localized in vivo 13C-NMR of glutamate metabolism in the human brain: initial results at 4 Tesla. Dev Neurosci (1998) 20:380–8. doi:10.1159/000017334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Rothman DL, Sibson NR, Hyder F, Shen J, Behar KL, Shulman RG. In vivo nuclear magnetic resonance spectroscopy studies of the relationship between the glutamate – glutamine neurotransmitter cycle and functional neuroenergetics. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci (1999) 354:1165–77. doi:10.1098/rstb.1999.0472

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Gruetter R, Seaquist ER, Ugurbil K. A mathematical model of compartmentalized neurotransmitter metabolism in the human brain. Am J Physiol Endocrinol Metab (2001) 281:E100–12. doi:10.1152/ajpendo.2001.281.1.E100

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Lebon V, Petersen KF, Cline GW, Shen J, Mason GF, Dufour S, et al. Astroglial contribution to brain energy metabolism in humans revealed by 13C nuclear magnetic resonance spectroscopy: elucidation of the dominant pathway for neurotransmitter glutamate repletion and measurement of astrocytic oxidative metabolism. J Neurosci (2002) 22:1523–31.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

29. Mason GF, Petersen KF, De Graaf RA, Shulman GI, Rothman DL. Measurements of the anaplerotic rate in the human cerebral cortex using 13C magnetic resonance spectroscopy and [1-13C] and [2-13C] glucose. J Neurochem (2007) 100:73–86. doi:10.1111/j.1471-4159.2006.04200.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Boumezbeur F, Mason GF, De Graaf RA, Behar KL, Cline GW, Shulman GI, et al. Altered brain mitochondrial metabolism in healthy aging as assessed by in vivo magnetic resonance spectroscopy. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab (2010) 30:211–21. doi:10.1038/jcbfm.2009.197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Ip IB, Berrington A, Hess AT, Parker AJ, Emir UE, Bridge H. Combined fMRI-MRS acquires simultaneous glutamate and BOLD-fMRI signals in the human brain. Neuroimage (2017) 155:113–9. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.04.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Goldman-Rakic P. Cellular basis of working memory. Neuron (1995) 14:477–85. doi:10.1016/0896-6273(95)90304-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Wang M, Yang Y, Wang C-J, Gamo NJ, Jin LE, Mazer JA, et al. NMDA receptors subserve persistent neuronal firing during working memory in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Neuron (2013) 77:736–49. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.12.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Michels L, Martin E, Klaver P, Edden R, Zelaya F, Lythgoe DJ, et al. Frontal GABA levels change during working memory. PLoS One (2012) 7:e31933. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0031933

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Owen AM, Mcmillan KM, Laird AR, Bullmore E. N-back working memory paradigm: a meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp (2005) 25:46–59. doi:10.1002/hbm.20131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Lynn J, Woodcock EA, Anand C, Khatib D, Stanley JA. Differences in steady-state glutamate levels and variability between ‘non-task-active’ conditions: Evidence from ¹H fMRS of the prefrontal cortex. NeuroImage (2018) 172:554–61. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.01.069

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Tkáć I, Gruetter R. Methodology of 1H NMR spectroscopy of the human brain at very high magnetic fields. Appl Magn Reson (2005) 29:139–57. doi:10.1007/BF03166960

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Woodcock EA, Arshad M, Khatib D, Stanley JA. Automated Voxel Placement: A Linux-Based Suite of Tools for Accurate and Reliable Single Voxel Coregistration. J Neuroimaging Psychiatry Neurol (2018).

Google Scholar

39. Provencher SW. LCModel User’s Manual, version 6.2. (2008). Available from: http://s-provencher.com/pages/lcm-manual.shtml

Google Scholar

40. De Graaf RA. In Vivo NMR Spectroscopy: Principles and Techniques. West Sussex: John Wiley & Sons (2013).

Google Scholar

41. Klose U. In vivo proton spectroscopy in presence of eddy currents. Magn Reson Med (1990) 14:26–30. doi:10.1002/mrm.1910140104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Dale A, Fischl B, Sereno M. Cortical surface-based analysis. I. Segmentation and surface reconstruction. Neuroimage (1999) 9:179–94. doi:10.1006/nimg.1998.0395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Smith S, Jenkinson M, Woolrich M, Beckmann C, Behrens T, Johansen-Berg H, et al. Advances in functional and structural MR image analysis and implementation as FSL. Neuroimage (2004) 23(Suppl 1):S208–19. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Stanley JA, Drost DJ, Williamson PC, Thompson RT. The use of a priori knowledge to quantify short echo in vivo 1H MR spectra. Magn Reson Med (1995) 34:17–24. doi:10.1002/mrm.1910340105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Cohen J. A power primer. Psychol Bull (1992) 112:155. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Erdfelder E, Faul F, Buchner A. GPOWER: a general power analysis program. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput (1996) 28:1–11. doi:10.3758/BF03203630

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Cavassila S, Deval S, Huegen C, Van Ormondt D, Graveron-Demilly D. Cramér-Rao bounds: an evaluation tool for quantitation. NMR Biomed (2001) 14:278–83. doi:10.1002/nbm.701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Rothman DL, Behar KL, Hyder F, Shulman RG. In vivo NMR studies of the glutamate neurotransmitter flux and neuroenergetics: implications for brain function. Annu Rev Physiol (2003) 65:401–27. doi:10.1146/annurev.physiol.65.092101.142131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Patel AB, De Graaf RA, Mason GF, Kanamatsu T, Rothman DL, Shulman RG, et al. Glutamatergic neurotransmission and neuronal glucose oxidation are coupled during intense neuronal activation. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab (2004) 24:972–85. doi:10.1097/01.WCB.0000126234.16188.71

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Sonnay S, Duarte JM, Just N, Gruetter R. Compartmentalised energy metabolism supporting glutamatergic neurotransmission in response to increased activity in the rat cerebral cortex: a 13C MRS study in vivo at 14.1 T. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab (2016) 36:928–40. doi:10.1177/0271678X16629482

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Mangia S, Giove F, Dinuzzo M. Metabolic pathways and activity-dependent modulation of glutamate concentration in the human brain. Neurochem Res (2012) 37:2554–61. doi:10.1007/s11064-012-0848-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Cole MW, Schneider W. The cognitive control network: integrated cortical regions with dissociable functions. Neuroimage (2007) 37:343–60. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Banich MT. Executive function: the search for an integrated account. Curr Dir Psychol Sci (2009) 18:89–94. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2009.01615.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2005) 102:9673–8. doi:10.1073/pnas.0504136102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Damoiseaux J, Rombouts S, Barkhof F, Scheltens P, Stam C, Smith SM, et al. Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2006) 103:13848–53. doi:10.1073/pnas.0601417103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Tatti R, Haley MS, Swanson OK, Tselha T, Maffei A. Neurophysiology and regulation of the balance between excitation and inhibition in neocortical circuits. Biol Psychiatry (2017) 81:821–31. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.09.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Hahn EL. Spin echoes. Phys Rev (1950) 80:580. doi:10.1103/PhysRev.80.580

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Wyss PO, Bianchini C, Scheidegger M, Giapitzakis IA, Hock A, Fuchs A, et al. In vivo estimation of transverse relaxation time constant (T2) of 17 human brain metabolites at 3T. Magn Reson Med (2018). doi:10.1002/mrm.27067

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Baslow MH, Hrabe J, Guilfoyle DN. Dynamic relationship between neurostimulation and N-acetylaspartate metabolism in the human visual cortex. J Mol Neurosci (2007) 32:235–45. doi:10.1007/s12031-007-0049-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Castellano G, Dias C, Foerster B, Li L, Covolan R. NAA and NAAG variation in neuronal activation during visual stimulation. Braz J Med Biol Res (2012) 45:1031–6. doi:10.1590/S0100-879X2012007500128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Landim RC, Edden RA, Foerster B, Li LM, Covolan RJ, Castellano G. Investigation of NAA and NAAG dynamics underlying visual stimulation using MEGA-PRESS in a functional MRS experiment. Magn Reson Imaging (2016) 34:239–45. doi:10.1016/j.mri.2015.10.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: magnetic resonance spectroscopy, glutamate, working memory, excitatory neurotransmission, metabolism, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, neuroimaging

Citation: Woodcock EA, Anand C, Khatib D, Diwadkar VA and Stanley JA (2018) Working Memory Modulates Glutamate Levels in the Dorsolateral Prefrontal Cortex during 1H fMRS. Front. Psychiatry 9:66. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00066

Received: 20 December 2017; Accepted: 19 February 2018;
Published: 06 March 2018

Edited by:

Paul Croarkin, Mayo Clinic Minnesota, United States

Reviewed by:

Frank P. MacMaster, University of Calgary, Canada
Meredith A. Reid, Auburn University, United States
Jeylan Close, University of Michigan, United States

Copyright: © 2018 Woodcock, Anand, Khatib, Diwadkar and Stanley. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jeffrey A. Stanley, jeffrey.stanley@wayne.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.