Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 11 May 2018
Sec. Psychopathology

An Examination of the Multi-Faceted Motivation System in Healthy Young Adults

  • 1Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 2Institute of Medical Science, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada
  • 3Department of Psychology, University of Toronto Scarborough, Scarborough, ON, Canada
  • 4Department of Psychiatry, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada

Background: Amotivation is a prevalent symptom in schizophrenia (SZ) and depression (MDD), and is linked to poor functional outcomes in affected individuals. Conceptualizations of motivation have outlined a multi-faceted construct comprised of reward responsiveness, reward expectancy, reward valuation, effort valuation, and action selection/preference-based decision making. To date, findings from studies utilizing variable-centered approaches to examining isolated facets of motivation in SZ and MDD have been inconsistent. Thus, the present study adopted a person-centered approach, and comprehensively examined the reward system in a non-clinical sample in an attempt to explore potential subtypes of motivation impairments, while minimizing the effects of illness-related confounds.

Methods: Ninety-six healthy undergraduate students were evaluated for amotivation, schizotypal traits, depressive symptoms, and cognition, and administered objective computerized tasks to measure the different facets of motivation. Cluster analysis was performed to explore subgroups of individuals based on similar motivation task performance. Additionally, correlational analyses were conducted in order to examine inter-relationships between motivation facets, and relations between clinical measures and facets of motivation.

Results: Cluster analysis identified two subgroups of individuals with differential motivation performance profiles. Correlational analyses revealed that reward responsiveness was associated with amotivation, depressive symptoms, and negative schizotypy. Further, significant inter-correlations were found between reward responsiveness and reward expectancy, as well as between reward valuation and effort valuation.

Conclusions: Our results mark important steps forward in understanding motivation in a non-clinical sample, and guide future dimensional and comprehensive analyses of the multi-faceted reward system. It remains to be seen whether these patterns of results will be similar in clinical populations such as SZ and MDD.

Introduction

Amotivation, a reduction in the ability to initiate and/or sustain goal-directed behavior, is a prevalent symptom in a number of neuropsychiatric illnesses such as schizophrenia (SZ) and major depressive disorder (MDD), and is inextricably linked to poor functional outcomes in affected individuals (14). Recognizing the importance of better understanding this symptom, the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) identified approach motivation as a fundamental dimension of behavior that cuts across diagnostic boundaries (5). Within RDoC, the Positive Valence Systems domain outlines multiple components of motivation including reward responsiveness, reward expectancy, reward valuation, effort valuation/willingness to work, and action selection/preference-based decision making (6). Reward responsiveness and reward expectancy refer to the in-the-moment and anticipated pleasure processes that occur in response to rewarding stimuli and cues. These hedonic processes help form internal representations of pleasure that serve to inform the appraisal and assignment of value to prospective rewards. The valuation of reward is then weighed against an effort-cost computation to determine if the reward is worth the effort as well as one's willingness to work for that reward. This cost-benefit analysis serves to guide not only decision making in the context of multiple choices, but also the subsequent execution of an action plan toward a final goal. These components align closely with frameworks that have emerged for the conceptualization of motivation as it pertains to psychiatric illnesses including SZ and MDD. For instance, in describing the motivational processes that contribute to goal-directed behavior, Barch and Dowd (7) suggest that hedonics or “liking” (i.e., reward responsiveness), and reward prediction and “wanting” (i.e., reward expectancy) interact to inform both reward valuation and effort valuation, leading to the development and implementation of an action plan (i.e., action selection/preference-based decision making) in order to achieve a desired goal.

To date, most studies examining motivation in SZ and MDD have focused on isolated facets of the motivation framework, typically within a single illness population. Accordingly, findings have emerged from behavioral and neuroimaging studies to suggest that patients with SZ exhibit impairments in reward valuation, reward expectancy, effort valuation, and action selection/decision making, but intact reward responsiveness (79). In contrast, patients with MDD have been shown to demonstrate impaired reward responsiveness, effort valuation, and action selection/decision making (8, 10). Given the heterogeneous nature of these illnesses, however, it is perhaps not surprising that these findings have been inconsistent across studies [see (8) for a review], with discrepancies often attributed to differences in cognitive functioning (i.e., memory, recall), medication status (e.g., atypical vs. typical antipsychotics), and symptom severity (i.e., anhedonia, negative symptoms) across clinical samples (8, 1117). With schizotypal and depressive traits in non-clinical populations positioned on continua that extend to their respective clinical populations (1820), investigating the multi-faceted motivation system in a non-clinical sample may afford insights into some of the processes related to clinical amotivation in schizophrenia and depression, while minimizing these potential illness-related confounds (21).

In addition to the heterogeneity within these patient samples, the variable findings across studies may also be a reflection of the multi-faceted nature of motivation itself, whereby impairments in any one or more of its underlying components can lead to the presentation of motivation deficits. To this end, these studies may be limited by their reliance on variable-centered approaches to examining isolated facets of motivation in that they do not address the heterogeneity existing within diagnostic groups, and fail to account for the possibility of different subtypes of motivation deficits. With this in mind, comprehensively examining the multiple components of motivation and adopting a person-centered approach may be well-suited to capture these individual differences, and resolve the multi-component heterogeneity of motivation (22). To date, however, no single study has yet to concurrently examine the multiple facets that comprise the motivation system in a population of healthy young adults, with only two studies, to our knowledge, examining effort valuation in a non-clinical sample (23, 24), and the majority of the remaining literature focusing on anhedonia (i.e., impaired reward responsiveness) in separate subclinical schizotypal or depressive samples (23, 2527). Further, the extent to which the multiple facets of the motivation system interact along a continuum of natural variations in levels of schizotypal and depressive symptoms remains unknown.

Thus, the present study aimed to comprehensively explore dimensions of the multi-faceted motivation and reward system in a healthy undergraduate sample using a person-centered approach. Specifically, we sought to identify individuals based on similarities in performance across motivation tasks, hypothesizing the emergence of underlying homogeneous subgroups with differential motivation profiles. We subsequently evaluated differences between these behavioral profiles on measures of subclinical schizotypal and depressive symptoms. In addition, we investigated the inter-relationships between motivation facets, as well as their demographic, clinical, and cognitive correlates in order to elucidate the processes involved in performance on these tasks.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A total of 117 undergraduate students at the University of Toronto Scarborough were recruited for this study through an online experiment registry for students in undergraduate psychology courses, and voluntarily participated for course credit. Participants were between the ages of 17–32, fluent in English, and capable of providing informed consent. Individuals were excluded from participation if they reported: taking any psychotropic medications; a current or past diagnosis of a schizophrenia-spectrum illness; a history of substance abuse or dependence within the past 6 months; or a history of neurological disease. This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Tri-Council Policy Statement, University of Toronto Research Ethics Board. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the University of Toronto Research Ethics Board. Participation in the study involved a single visit to a psychology laboratory at the university where an experimenter provided instructions for, and administered a series of computerized self-report and behavioral measures.

Measures

Self-Report Measures

All participants were administered the Personality Assessment Inventory (PAI), a 344-item diagnostic assessment to screen for psychopathology (28), the self-report version of the Apathy Evaluation Scale [AES-S; (29)] as a measure of clinical amotivation, and the Temporal Experience of Pleasure Scale [TEPS; (30)] to evaluate consummatory (TEPS-Con) and anticipatory (TEPS-Ant) pleasure. The Likert-scale version of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire [SPQ; (31, 32)] was administered to assess for schizotypal traits, from which positive (SPQ-Pos), negative (SPQ-Neg), and disorganized (SPQ-Dis) schizotypy subscores were also calculated. In addition, depressive symptoms were assessed using the Centre for Epidemiologic Studies–Depression Scale [CES-D; (33)]. Lastly, global cognitive functioning was assessed using the Brief Neurocognitive Assessment, consisting of tests of working memory and processing speed (34).

Objective Computerized Measures

A series of objective computerized tasks, informed by the RDoC Positive Valence System matrix (35), were subsequently administered to measure each facet of the motivation system. Participants were informed that all monetary rewards were hypothetical, but were instructed to play as though they would actually receive the money. For each task, outcome variables were converted into a single standardized z-score to represent the separate facets of motivation. Task administration was also randomized to minimize order effects. Only brief explanations of each task are presented below given that detailed descriptions, including specific instructions, have been previously published by their respective authors.

Reward Responsiveness

The International Affective Picture System (IAPS), which has been extensively used in a number of neuropsychiatric illnesses including SZ and MDD, and in healthy adult samples (36, 37), was administered to assess for reward responsiveness or “liking” (36). In the current study, participants were presented with 42 positive, neutral and negative valence images1 (14 each), and instructed to rate in-the-moment pleasantness (IAPS-Pleasant) and arousal (IAPS-Arousal) on a 9-point Likert scale (Figure 1). Given our interest in approach motivation and reward-driven processes, we focused our analyses on positive-valence images only, with higher pleasantness and arousal ratings reflecting greater capacity to experience pleasure in response to positive stimuli. A composite score of the standardized IAPS-Pleasant and IAPS-Arousal variables was computed as a measure of reward responsiveness (IAPS-Composite).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic representation of a single trial in the International Affective Picture Rating System (IAPS) task (36). In this example, pleasantness and arousal ratings must be made for a positive-valence image.

Reward Expectancy

The Cued Reinforcement Reaction Time (CRRT) task was used to assess for reward expectancy and “wanting” (38). The CRRT has been administered in SZ and healthy control (HC) samples (38, 39). Briefly, the CRRT requires rapid “odd-one-out” judgments on three circles across 96 trials. In each trial, the presentation of the circles is preceded by a cue with different colored (i.e., red, blue, or yellow) frames (Figure 2). Unbeknownst to the participant, each colored frame represents a differentially reinforced cue, such that points are awarded for correct responses on 90% (red frame), 50% (blue frame), and 10% (yellow frame) of the trials. However, participants are informed that the likelihood of receiving points is predetermined according to the color of the frame, and that they will receive 100 points for fast and correct responses, 1 point for slow and correct responses, and 0 points for incorrect responses. Accordingly, individuals should respond fastest when they anticipate a reward, particularly on the highest reinforcement probability (i.e., 90%) trials. Two practice blocks are administered before the task with the first to ensure task comprehension, and the second for calibrating individual reaction times. Specifically, the cut-off for a fast response was individually determined for each participant by subtracting one standard deviation from their median reaction time (RT) in the second practice block. The variable of interest is anticipated reinforcement-related speeding (CRRT-RRS) which represents the degree to which participants modulate their behavior (i.e., reaction time) in response to reward cues. CRRT-RSS was calculated by subtracting the median RT for low probability trials from the median RT for high probability trials, with smaller values are indicative of higher levels of RRS, or a greater degree of wanting.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Schematic depicting the trial sequence in the Cued Reinforcement Reaction Time (CRRT) task (38). In this example, 100 points are awarded for a fast response on a high probability reinforcement trial.

Reward Valuation

Reward valuation was assessed using the Kirby Delay Discounting (Kirby DD) task which measures participants' differential valuation of monetary rewards over time (40). The Kirby task has been used in a number of neuropsychiatric disorders, including SZ and MDD, as well as in healthy undergraduate samples (41, 42). In this task, participants are instructed to choose between a small immediate reward and a larger delayed reward (over varying number of days) across 27 trials (Figure 3). The outcome of interest is the rate at which participants discount the value of future rewards, calculated as the average natural logarithm (ln) of the discounting rate (Kirby–Avg). Smaller values are indicative of lower discounting rates, or greater reward valuation.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Examples of items utilized in the Kirby Delay Discounting questionnaire [Kirby DD; (40)].

Effort Valuation

Effort valuation was assessed using the Effort Expenditure for Rewards Task (EEfRT), which measures participants' willingness to expend effort for monetary gains (24). The EEfRT is one of the most commonly used measures of effort-based decision making, and has been used in SZ, MDD, and undergraduate samples (23, 24, 43, 44). Participants are presented with the opportunity to receive a reward by choosing between two effortful button-pressing tasks that differ in level of difficulty. Specifically, the easy task requires 30 button presses (in 7 s) with the index finger of the dominant hand, whereas the hard task requires 100 button presses (in 21 s) with the pinky finger of the non-dominant hand. For each trial, participants are informed of the magnitude of the reward for the easy vs. hard task (ranging from $1.00 to $4.85), as well as the probability of receiving that reward (i.e., 12, 50, or 88%) if they successfully complete the task (Figure 4). On reinforced trials, the reward for successfully completing the easy task was always $1.00, whereas the reward for a successful completion of the hard task ranged from $1.24 to $4.21. Thus, for each trial, participants must compute a cost-benefit analysis based on task difficulty, reward magnitude and reward probability, which ultimately serves to guide their decision to expend effort. Given the impairments typically observed in clinical populations at the high probability condition, we specifically examined the proportion of hard tasks chosen at the 88% reward level (EEfRT-High), thus evaluating willingness to work when the likelihood of reward is most certain and least ambiguous.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Schematic depicting the trial sequence in the Effort Expenditure for Rewards Task [EEfRT; (24)]. In this example, the hard task was selected and successfully completed on a high reward magnitude and high probability trial.

Action Selection/Decision Making

Lastly, action selection/decision making was assessed using the Multitasking in the City Test [MCT; (45)], a virtual reality errand-running task which has been used in SZ (46) and non-clinical samples (45). In this task, participants are provided a map of the virtual city, as well as a list of eight pre-specified errands (e.g., buy groceries, attend a doctor's appointment, etc.) that must be completed within a period of 15 min. Participants navigate through the city using a joystick and are required to visit different stores in order to complete the required errands (Figure 5). Throughout the task, the list of completed and uncompleted errands is displayed on-screen to minimize the cognitive load. Participants are also provided feedback after each attempt at completing an errand, and must utilize this information to guide efficient decision making and goal-directed behavior (47). The MCT outcome variables are the total distance traveled (MCT-Distance; in virtual environment units—VEUs) and performance score (MCT-Performance), calculated as the total number of tasks completed minus total omission and commission errors (i.e., tasks failed or repeated, respectively). Shorter distances and higher performance scores indicate better decision making and planning. With MCT-Performance reflecting the decision making process, and MCT-Distance as an index of action selection and implementation, an MCT composite score was computed to represent the unified construct of action selection/decision making.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Screenshots of the virtual city in the Multitasking in the City Test [MCT; (45)].

All self-report scales and objective motivation tasks were programmed and administered on Open Sesame v. 2.9.4 (48), except for the MCT which was run on a stand-alone software for Microsoft Windows. All computer tasks were presented on a 30′′ LCD display, with the participant providing input for each task either with the keyboard (for the IAPS, EEfRT, etc.) or with a standard video game controller (MCT).

Analyses

Validity

Participants with clinically significant scores on any of the PAI clinical scales, as per Morey's guidelines (28) were excluded from all subsequent analyses (n = 5). Further, due to technological problems, an additional 16 participants with incomplete data in one or more of the motivation task were excluded, bringing the final sample to 96 participants. Sample demographics and symptom characteristics are shown in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Sample demographics and symptom characterization.

In addition, the Dot Counting Test (DCT) was administered to assess for performance validity, such that participants with missing DCT data (n = 9), or a score greater than or equal to 14 (n = 10) were deemed invalid (49, 50). In an effort to conserve statistical power, analyses were first conducted with all 96 participants (results shown). The validity of these results was then confirmed by repeating the analyses (n = 77) after excluding participants who exerted non-credible findings.

Statistical Analyses

Cronbach's alpha was calculated for each self-report measure to ensure consistent responding across the entire sample. Cluster analysis, using the k-means algorithm, as per MacQueen's methodology (51) was performed on reward responsiveness (i.e., IAPS-Composite), reward expectancy (i.e., CRRT-RRS), reward valuation (i.e., Kirby-Avg), effort valuation (i.e., EEfRT-High), and action selection/decision making (i.e., MCT-Composite) in order to identify unique subgroups of individuals with similar motivation task performance. Cluster solution was determined using the “NbClust” package, which utilizes an exhaustive array of indices (e.g., Silhouette, Calinski, and Harabasz index, etc.) to determine the optimal number of clusters (52). The consistency of the cluster solution was confirmed by establishing consensus with a second clustering method (i.e., hierarchical agglomerative clustering). Independent samples t-tests were subsequently conducted to assess for differences in clinical measures and motivation performance between clusters. Lastly, inter-relationships between motivation facets, and bivariate relations between clinical measures and facets of motivation were examined using Spearman correlations due to non-normal variable distributions.

All analyses were conducted using the Statistical Package of Social Science (SPSS) version 24, with the exception of the cluster analysis which was conducted using the “fpc” package version 2.1-10 (53) in R version 3.3.3 (54).

Results

Consistency of Self-Report Clinical Measures

Cronbach's alpha for the AES (α = 0.77), CES-D (α = 0.89), SPQ (SPQ-Pos: α = 0.90; SPQ-Neg; α = 0.92; SPQ-Dis: α = 0.90), and TEPS-Ant: α = 0.73 revealed good reliability across scales, suggestive of consistent responding amongst participants. The reliability of TEPS-Con was lower with an alpha coefficient of 0.63, though still similar to other studies utilizing this scale in a non-clinical sample (30, 5557).

Cluster Analysis

The k-means clustering algorithm identified a 2 cluster solution with 49 participants assigned to cluster 1 and 47 to cluster 2. The consistency of the cluster solution was verified using hierarchical clustering which revealed an 83% overlap between the two methods. Significant group differences reveal impaired reward expectancy in cluster 1, with cluster 2 characterized by impaired reward valuation, effort valuation, and action selection/decision making (Table 2). In contrast, the clusters did not significantly differ on performance in reward responsiveness. The motivation performance profiles of the clusters are shown in Figure 6. Additionally, as shown in Table 2, clusters did not significantly differ by age, sex, level of education, or consummatory or anticipatory pleasure. Clinically, there were no differences in severity of amotivation, overall schizotypal traits, or in depressive symptoms, though there was a non-significant trend for differences in negative schizotypy (p = 0.06). Importantly, excluding non-credible participants did not change these results.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Demographic, clinical, and motivation performance characterization of clusters.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Motivation performance profiles across clusters. Significant differences are denoted by **p < 0.01. Error bars represent standard error of the mean.

Correlational Analyses

Bivariate correlations between clinical measures and motivation task performance are shown in Table 3. Interestingly, AES, CES-D, and SPQ-Neg were only correlated with reward responsiveness as measured by the IAPS-Composite score. Reward responsiveness was also positively correlated with both TEPS-Con and TEPS-Ant.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Bivariate correlations between clinical measures and motivation tasks.

Inter-Task Correlations

Correlations were also conducted to examine the interrelationships between motivation variables. As shown in Table 4, significant positive inter-task correlations were found between reward responsiveness and reward expectancy, and between reward valuation and effort valuation.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Inter-task correlations.

Discussion

Informed by the RDoC Positive Valence Systems domain (6), the present study aimed to comprehensively explore the multiple facets of the motivation and reward system in a non-clinical sample using a battery of objective computerized tasks. Drawing from dimensional approaches to understanding psychopathology (5), we specifically utilized a person-centered approach to identify subgroups of individuals based on motivation task performance. The results of our cluster analyses revealed two groups of individuals with distinct behavioral profiles, such that the first cluster demonstrated impairments in reward expectancy, whereas cluster 2 was characterized by impaired reward valuation, effort valuation, and action selection/decision making. Interestingly, however, reward responsiveness was comparable between clusters, perhaps a reflection of the relatively low levels of amotivation endorsed by these individuals. In fact, a study by our group examined anhedonia in SZ and HC participants and found that it was only the individuals with high levels of amotivation (i.e., AES scores > 40) who demonstrated impaired reward responsiveness (12). Thus, the absence of a cluster difference in reward responsiveness may in part be a result of the restricted severity of amotivation in the present sample, which may not translate into impairments in this specific facet of motivation. Further, clusters did not significantly differ in age, sex, cognitive functioning, depressive symptoms, or overall schizotypal traits. There was, however, a non-significant trend for differences in negative schizotypy between clusters, such that individuals in cluster 1 endorsed higher levels of negative schizotypal traits compared to those in cluster 2. Given that cluster 1 was characterized by impaired reward expectancy, this elevation in negative schizotypy seems to fit well with other studies similarly revealing reduced anticipatory pleasure in individuals with negative schizotypal symptoms (27, 5860). Nonetheless, the absence of significant differences between clusters is particularly important as it suggests that individuals are not simply being clustered by symptom severity or cognitive functioning, but rather by similar performance on discrete facets of motivation. Interestingly, clusters also presented with comparable levels of clinical amotivation, further highlighting the multi-faceted nature and varying behavioral underpinnings of motivational impairments.

Correlational analyses revealed that reward responsiveness, was related to amotivation, negative schizotypal traits and depressive symptoms, such that higher scores on the IAPS was associated with lower symptomatology. These findings are in line with other studies that have demonstrated a relationship between overall negative symptoms and pleasure in non-clinical samples (25, 30, 61). Interestingly, the MCT composite score was also positively associated with anticipatory pleasure, perhaps indicative of the hedonic processes involved in preference-based decision making and goal-driven action selection. The overall lack of significant correlations between symptom measures and facets of motivation, however, may be a reflection of the lower symptom severity in this sample, with more robust and widespread relationships emerging in clinical populations such as SZ and MDD that experience more severe impairments.

Correlational analyses were also conducted to explore the inter-relationships between the objective motivation measures. Our results revealed a significant relationship between reward responsiveness (i.e., “liking”) and reward expectancy (i.e., “wanting”). These findings are in line with the literature on anhedonia in SZ and MDD which distinguishes between the separate, but inter-related consummatory and anticipatory components of hedonic experience (30, 62). Further, the significant correlation between reward valuation and effort valuation fits well with the current conceptualizations of the motivation framework, with reward valuation and effort valuation subsumed into one construct representing cost-benefit decision making (7). Thus, our findings lend further support to the inter-related reward and effort valuation processes that serve to inform cost-benefit analyses in the context of rewards and goals. Overall, however, the small and limited inter-correlations across these facets suggest that they are indeed distinct and are likely tapping into different domains of the motivation system.

There are limitations to this study that warrant mention. First, our study sample consisted entirely of relatively young university students, which limits the generalizability of these results to other populations. Further, although participants were excluded if they reported a history of psychiatric illness, a formal diagnostic interview was not conducted to confirm the absence of any current or past disorders. However, we did utilize the PAI, a valid self-report measure of psychopathology, and excluded participants with clinically significant scores. The subjective nature of symptom measures serves as another limitation of this study, though we did include objective measures of motivation to augment these clinical rating scales, and administered the DCT in order to minimize non-credible responding secondary to poor effort. In addition, our computerized measures of motivation utilized different types of intangible rewards including pleasant imagery, points, and hypothetical monetary reward; however, a number of studies have failed to find differences between hypothetical (i.e., abstract) and real (i.e., tangible) rewards both in terms of behavioral performance and resultant brain activity during these tasks (6365). It is also important to note that cluster analyses, though widely used, are exploratory and no standard method exists for determining the optimal number of clusters. While subjective judgment is often required, we took additional steps to validate our results by establishing consensus amongst an exhaustive list of indices, and with a different clustering method. Moreover, while there are no statistical guidelines for calculating minimum sample size requirements to conduct cluster analysis, our sample size exceeded Formann and Kohlmann's (66) recommendation of a minimum sample size of 2k where k equals the number of clustering variables. That said, it is still possible that our sample size may have restricted the number of clusters identified, and it will therefore be important to replicate our findings in larger groups. Finally, the comprehensive evaluation of discrete facets that comprise the motivational system would benefit from inclusion of multiple reward-based tasks believed to tap into overlapping constructs, although to our knowledge the present investigation represents the most extensive to date.

The findings of the present study revealed two clusters of individuals with differential motivation performance profiles. Going forward, it will be important to investigate the underlying neural circuits distinguishing these profiles. Though speculative, the reward anticipation deficits in cluster 1 may be related to striatal dysfunction (8, 67), with the impairments seen in cluster 2 potentially associated with higher-order cortical regions of the brain (8, 13). Further, it remains to be seen whether this pattern of results will be similar in SZ and MDD populations with more severe symptoms and motivational impairments. Our results also provide further support for the current conceptualization of motivation as a multi-faceted framework comprised of distinct, but interrelated components. Taken together, our findings align with recent dimensional approaches to understanding core domains of psychopathology, and highlight the need for continued comprehensive evaluations of the motivation system across a continuum of healthy and clinical populations.

Author Contributions

GF and AA: Designed the study; SD: Led the statistical analyses and preparation of the first draft of the manuscript; AA, SS, and IS: Contributed to the data collection and statistical analyses. All other authors subsequently made significant contributions to the interpretation of the findings, and have approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This work was funded through an Ontario Mental Health Foundation research grant (GF). This source had no further role in the analysis or interpretation of the data, development of the manuscript or the decision to submit for publication.

Conflict of Interest Statement

ZJD has received research support from Brainsway Inc. and Magventure Inc., has served on the advisory board for Sunovion, Hoffmann-La Roche Limited and Merck, and has received speaker support from Eli Lilly. GR has received consultant fees from Neurocrine Biosciences and Synchroneuron, as well as research support from Novartis. GF has served as an investigator on research sponsored by Medicure Inc., and Neurocrine Bioscience. He has also served on advisory boards for Hoffman-La Roche and Takeda, and received speaker's fees from Hoffman-La Roche, Lundbeck and Novartis.

The other authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank Drs. Roshan Cools and Hanneke den Ouden for sharing the CRRT with us for this study. We would also like to thank the participants who volunteered to take part in this study.

Footnotes

1. ^IAPS stimuli used in the study included: 1052, 1120, 1300, 1390, 1450, 1560, 1602, 1620, 2270, 2304, 2320, 2360, 2370, 2480, 2490, 2495, 2590, 2722, 4598, 5779, 5891, 5920, 5950, 5971, 6260, 6560, 7325, 7640, 8030, 8080, 8160, 8180, 8185, 8370, 8400, 8490, 9001, 9210, 9220, 9250, 9280, 9331.

References

1. Fervaha G, Foussias G, Takeuchi H, Agid O, Remington G. Motivational deficits in major depressive disorder: cross-sectional and longitudinal relationships with functional impairment and subjective well-being. Compr Psychiatry (2016) 66:31–8. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2015.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Foussias G, Mann S, Zakzanis KK, van Reekum R, Remington G. Motivational deficits as the central link to functioning in schizophrenia: a pilot study. Schizophr Res. (2009) 115:333–7. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.09.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Kiang M, Christensen BK, Remington G, Kapur S. Apathy in schizophrenia: clinical correlates and association with functional outcome. Schizophr Res. (2003) 63:79–88. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(02)00433-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Wells KB, Stewart A, Hays RD, Burnam MA, Rogers W, Daniels M, et al. The functioning and well-being of depressed patients. Results from the Medical Outcomes Study. JAMA (1989) 262:914–9. doi: 10.1001/jama.1989.03430070062031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Insel T, Cuthbert B, Garvey M, Heinssen R, Pine DS, Quinn K, et al. Research domain criteria (RDoC): toward a new classification framework for research on mental disorders. Am J Psychiatry (2010) 167:748–51. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09091379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Cuthbert BN, Insel TR. Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: the seven pillars of RDoC. BMC Med. (2013) 11:126. doi: 10.1186/1741-7015-11-126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Barch DM, Dowd EC. Goal representations and motivational drive in schizophrenia: the role of prefrontal–striatal interactions. Schizophr Bull. (2010) 36:919–34. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbq068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Barch DM, Pagliaccio D, Luking K. Mechanisms underlying motivational deficits in psychopathology: similarities and differences in depression and schizophrenia. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. (2016) 27:411–49. doi: 10.1007/7854_2015_376

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Kring AM, Barch DM. The motivation and pleasure dimension of negative symptoms: neural substrates and behavioral outputs. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol J Eur Coll Neuropsychopharmacol. (2014) 24:725–36. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2013.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Treadway MT, Zald DH. Reconsidering anhedonia in depression: lessons from translational neuroscience. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2011) 35:537–55. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Beninger RJ, Wasserman J, Zanibbi K, Charbonneau D, Mangels J, Beninger BV. Typical and atypical antipsychotic medications differentially affect two nondeclarative memory tasks in schizophrenic patients: a double dissociation. Schizophr Res. (2003) 61:281–92. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(02)00315-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Da Silva S, Saperia S, Siddiqui I, Fervaha G, Agid O, Daskalakis ZJ, et al. Investigating consummatory and anticipatory pleasure across motivation deficits in schizophrenia and healthy controls. Psychiatry Res. (2017) 254:112–7. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.04.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Gold JM, Waltz JA, Frank MJ. Effort cost computation in schizophrenia: a commentary on the recent literature. Biol Psychiatry (2015) 78:747–53. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Juckel G, Schlagenhauf F, Koslowski M, Filonov D, Wüstenberg T, Villringer A, et al. Dysfunction of ventral striatal reward prediction in schizophrenic patients treated with typical, not atypical, neuroleptics. Psychopharmacology (2006) 187:222–8. doi: 10.1007/s00213-006-0405-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Treadway MT, Bossaller N, Shelton RC, Zald DH. Effort-based decision-making in major depressive disorder: a translational model of motivational anhedonia. J Abnorm Psychol. (2012) 121:553–8. doi: 10.1037/a0028813

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Pizzagalli DA, Iosifescu D, Hallett LA, Ratner KG, Fava M. Reduced hedonic capacity in major depressive disorder: evidence from a probabilistic reward task. J Psychiatr Res. (2008) 43:76–87. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2008.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Strauss GP, Gold JM. A new perspective on anhedonia in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry (2012) 169:364–73. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11030447

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Kendler KS, Gardner CO. Boundaries of major depression: an evaluation of DSM-IV criteria. Am J Psychiatry (1998) 155:172–7.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

19. Linscott RJ, van Os J. An updated and conservative systematic review and meta-analysis of epidemiological evidence on psychotic experiences in children and adults: on the pathway from proneness to persistence to dimensional expression across mental disorders. Psychol Med. (2013) 43:1133–49. doi: 10.1017/S0033291712001626

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. van Os J, Hanssen M, Bijl RV, Ravelli A. Strauss (1969) revisited: a psychosis continuum in the general population? Schizophr Res. (2000) 45:11–20. doi: 10.1016/S0920-9964(99)00224-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Cohen AS, Mohr C, Ettinger U, Chan RCK, Park S. Schizotypy as an organizing framework for social and affective sciences. Schizophr Bull. (2015) 41(Suppl 2):S427–35. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbu195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Bergman LR, Andersson H. The person and the variable in developmental psychology. Z Für Psychol J Psychol. (2010) 218:155–65. doi: 10.1027/0044-3409/a000025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. McCarthy JM, Treadway MT, Blanchard JJ. Motivation and effort in individuals with social anhedonia. Schizophr Res. (2015) 165:70–5. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.03.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Treadway MT, Buckholtz JW, Schwartzman AN, Lambert WE, Zald DH. Worth the ‘EEfRT’? The effort expenditure for rewards task as an objective measure of motivation and anhedonia. PLoS ONE (2009) 4:e6598. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006598

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Chan RCK, Wang Y, Yan C, Zhao Q, McGrath J, Hsi X, et al. A study of trait anhedonia in non-clinical Chinese samples: evidence from the chapman scales for physical and social anhedonia. PLoS ONE (2012) 7:e34275. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0034275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Liu W, Chan RCK, Wang L, Huang J, Cheung EFC, Gong Q, et al. Deficits in sustaining reward responses in subsyndromal and syndromal major depression. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2011) 35:1045–52. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2011.02.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Yan C, Liu WH, Cao Y, Chan RCK. Self-reported pleasure experience and motivation in individuals with schizotypal personality disorders proneness. East Asian Arch Psychiatry (2011) 21:115–22.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

28. Morey L. Personality Assessment Inventory: Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources (1991).

29. Marin RS, Biedrzycki RC, Firinciogullari S. Reliability and validity of the apathy evaluation scale. Psychiatry Res. (1991) 38:143–62. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(91)90040-V

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Gard DE, Gard MG, Kring AM, John OP. Anticipatory and consummatory components of the experience of pleasure: a scale development study. J Res Pers. (2006) 40:1086–102. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2005.11.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Raine A. The SPQ: a scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophr Bull. (1991) 17:555–64. doi: 10.1093/schbul/17.4.555

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Wuthrich V, Bates TC. Reliability and validity of two Likert versions of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ). Pers Individ Diff. (2005) 38:1543–8. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2004.09.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Radloff LS. The CES-D scale a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Appl Psychol Meas. (1977) 1:385–401. doi: 10.1177/014662167700100306

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Fervaha G, Agid O, Foussias G, Remington G. Toward a more parsimonious assessment of neurocognition in schizophrenia: a 10-minute assessment tool. J Psychiatr Res. (2014) 52:50–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. National Institute of Mental Health. RDoC Matrix (2013). Available online at: https://www.nimh.nih.gov/research-priorities/rdoc/constructs/rdoc-matrix.shtml

36. Lang PJ, Greenwald MK, Bradley MM, Hamm AO. Looking at pictures: affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions. Psychophysiology (1993) 30:261–73. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1993.tb03352.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Jayaro C, de la Vega I, Díaz-Marsá M, Montes A, Carrasco JL. [The use of the International Affective Picture System for the study of affective dysregulation in mental disorders]. Actas Esp Psiquiatr. (2008) 36:177–82.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

38. Cools R, Blackwell A, Clark L, Menzies L, Cox S, Robbins TW. Tryptophan depletion disrupts the motivational guidance of goal-directed behavior as a function of trait impulsivity. Neuropsychopharmacology (2005) 30:1362–73. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1300704

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Murray GK, Clark L, Corlett PR, Blackwell AD, Cools R, Jones PB, et al. Incentive motivation in first-episode psychosis: a behavioural study. BMC Psychiatry (2008) 8:34. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-8-34

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Kirby KN, Maraković NN. Delay-discounting probabilistic rewards: rates decrease as amounts increase. Psychon Bull Rev. (1996) 3:100–4. doi: 10.3758/BF03210748

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Heerey EA, Robinson BM, McMahon RP, Gold JM. Delay discounting in schizophrenia. Cogn Neuropsychiatry (2007) 12:213–21. doi: 10.1080/13546800601005900

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Pulcu E, Trotter PD, Thomas EJ, McFarquhar M, Juhasz G, Sahakian BJ, et al. Temporal discounting in major depressive disorder. Psychol Med. (2014) 44:1825–34. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713002584

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Fervaha G, Graff-Guerrero A, Zakzanis KK, Foussias G, Agid O, Remington G. Incentive motivation deficits in schizophrenia reflect effort computation impairments during cost-benefit decision-making. J Psychiatr Res. (2013) 47:1590–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2013.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Yang X-H, Huang J, Zhu C-Y, Wang Y-F, Cheung EFC, Chan RCK, et al. Motivational deficits in effort-based decision making in individuals with subsyndromal depression, first-episode and remitted depression patients. Psychiatry Res. (2014) 220:874–82. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.08.056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Jovanovski D, Zakzanis K, Campbell Z, Erb S, Nussbaum D. Development of a novel, ecologically oriented virtual reality measure of executive function: the Multitasking in the City Test. Appl Neuropsychol Adult (2012) 19:171–82. doi: 10.1080/09084282.2011.643955

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Siddiqui I, Fervaha G, Jeffay E, Zakzanis K, Agid O, Remington G, et al. Objective assessment of planning and executing goal-directed behaviour in schizophrenia using a virtual environment. Biol Psychiatry (2015) 77:S565.

47. Parsons TD. Virtual reality for enhanced ecological validity and experimental control in the clinical, affective and social neurosciences. Front Hum Neurosci. (2015) 9:660. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00660

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Mathôt S, Schreij D, Theeuwes J. OpenSesame: an open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behav Res Methods (2012) 44:314–24. doi: 10.3758/s13428-011-0168-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. An KY, Zakzanis KK, Joordens S. Conducting research with non-clinical healthy undergraduates: does effort play a role in neuropsychological test performance? Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2012) 27:849–57. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acs085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Boone KB, Lu P, Herzberg DS. The Dot Counting Test Manual. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services (2002).

51. MacQueen J. Some methods for classification and analysis of multivariate observations. In: Proceedings of the Fifth Berkeley Symposium on Mathematical Statistics and Probability. Oakland, CA. (1967) p. 281–97.

Google Scholar

52. Charrad M, Ghazzali N, Boiteau V, Niknafs A. NbClust: an R package for determining the relevant number of clusters in a data set. J Stat Softw. (2014) 61:1–36. doi: 10.18637/jss.v061.i06

CrossRef Full Text

53. Henning C. Fpc: Flexible Procedures for Clustering. R Package Version 2.1-10. (2015).

54. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing (2016).

55. Strauss GP, Wilbur RC, Warren KR, August SM, Gold JM. Anticipatory vs. consummatory pleasure: what is the nature of hedonic deficits in schizophrenia? Psychiatry Res. (2011) 187:36–41. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Cassidy CM, Lepage M, Harvey PO, Malla A. Cannabis use and anticipatory pleasure as reported by subjects with early psychosis and community controls. Schizophr Res. (2012) 137:39–44. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.02.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Geaney JT, Treadway MT, Smillie LD. Trait anticipatory pleasure predicts effort expenditure for reward. PLoS ONE (2015) 10:e0131357. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0131357

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Li Z, Lui SSY, Geng F, Li Y, Li W, Wang C, et al. Experiential pleasure deficits in different stages of schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. (2015) 166:98–103. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2015.05.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Shi Y, Wang Y, Cao X, Wang Y, Wang Y, Zong J, et al. Experience of pleasure and emotional expression in individuals with schizotypal personality features. PLoS ONE (2012) 7:e34147. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0034147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Fervaha G, Remington G. Invalid responding in questionnaire-based research: implications for the study of schizotypy. Psychol Assess. (2013) 25:1355–60. doi: 10.1037/a0033520

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Fervaha G, Zakzanis KK, Foussias G, Agid O, Remington G. Distress related to subclinical negative symptoms in a non-clinical sample: role of dysfunctional attitudes. Psychiatry Res. (2015) 230:249–54. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Klein D. Depression and anhedonia. In: Anhedonia and Affect Deficit States. New York, NY: PMA Publishing Corporations. (1984) p. 1–14.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

63. Bickel WK, Pitcock JA, Yi R, Edgardo JCA. Congruence of BOLD response across intertemporal choice conditions: fictive and real money gains and losses: behavioral and neural differences in discounting. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci. (2009) 29:8839–46. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5319-08.2009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Bowman CH, Turnbull OH. Real versus facsimile reinforcers on the Iowa Gambling Task. Brain Cogn. (2003) 53:207–10. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00111-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Lagorio CH, Madden GJ. Delay discounting of real and hypothetical rewards III: steady-state assessments, forced-choice trials, and all real rewards. Behav Process. (2005) 69:173–87. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2005.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Formann AK, Kohlmann T. Latent class analysis in medical research. Stat Methods Med Res. (1996) 5:179–211. doi: 10.1177/096228029600500205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Juckel G, Schlagenhauf F, Koslowski M, Wüstenberg T, Villringer A, Knutson B, Wrase J, et al. Dysfunction of ventral striatal reward prediction in schizophrenia. Neuroimage (2006) 29:409–16. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.07.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: amotivation, reward system, schizotypal traits, depressive symptoms, RDoC

Citation: Da Silva S, Apatsidou A, Saperia S, Siddiqui I, Jeffay E, Voineskos AN, Daskalakis ZJ, Remington G, Zakzanis KK and Foussias G (2018) An Examination of the Multi-Faceted Motivation System in Healthy Young Adults. Front. Psychiatry 9:191. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00191

Received: 15 December 2017; Accepted: 23 April 2018;
Published: 11 May 2018.

Edited by:

Xavier Noel, Free University of Brussels, Belgium

Reviewed by:

Henrik Gustafsson, Karlstad University, Sweden
Alvaro Sanchez-Lopez, Complutense University of Madrid, Spain

Copyright © 2018 Da Silva, Apatsidou, Saperia, Siddiqui, Jeffay, Voineskos, Daskalakis, Remington, Zakzanis and Foussias. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: George Foussias, george.foussias@camh.ca

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.