Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Public Health, 21 December 2017
Sec. Public Mental Health

Increased Physical Fitness Is Associated with Higher Executive Functioning in People with Dementia

  • 1Department of Behavioural Science and Health, University College London, London, United Kingdom
  • 2Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck College, University of London, London, United Kingdom

Physical fitness (PF) has been associated with improved cognition in older age, but less is known about its effects on different cognitive domains in individuals diagnosed with dementia. We explored the associations between PF and cognitive performance in 40 healthy elderly and 30 individuals with dementia. Participants completed a battery of standardized cognitive tests (Mini-Mental State Exam, Verbal Fluency, Prospective and Retrospective Memory Questionnaire, Clock Drawing, and California Verbal Learning Test) and were classified into high versus low levels of PF based on their score on the Physical Fitness Questionnaire. Analyses took into account age, gender, education, occupation, head injury, Internet use, brain training, and past levels of exercise and revealed overall benefits of PF, in particular for the people with dementia. Discriminant analysis showed high accuracy of reclassification, with most errors being due to the misclassification of dementia cases as healthy when they had high PF. The first discriminant function accounted for 83% of the variance. Using individual estimates of this function, which reflected global cognitive performance, confirmed the beneficial role of PF in dementia, even when taking into account age, past level of exercise, and the number of years since the dementia diagnosis. Finally, univariate analyses confirmed the differential sensitivity of the cognitive tests, with MMSE and clock drawing showing reliable interaction effects. This work shows that PF is associated with a reduced level of cognitive deterioration expected with dementia, especially in executive functioning and provides empirical support for the cognitive benefits of interventions promoting PF for individuals with dementia.

Highlight

Physical fitness is associated with higher global cognition in individuals with dementia, independent of past levels of exercise and illness duration.

Introduction

Dementia is one of the leading causes of disability in later life, ahead of cancer, cardiovascular disease, and stroke (1) and is characterized by a decline in many aspects of cognitive ability. Despite promising new research (2), there is currently no pharmacological cure for dementia (3). It is, therefore, important to help identify behavioral and environmental modifiable risk factors that could reduce the progression of the disease. One such risk factor is physical activity (PA), which is defined as any bodily movement produced by skeletal muscles that result in energy expenditure. PA differs from physical fitness (PF), which represents a set of attributes that people have or achieve that relates to the ability to perform PA.

Current recommendations from the World Health Organization (4) suggest that throughout the week, older adults (age 65+) should perform either at least 150 min of moderate-intensity aerobic PA or 75 min of vigorous-intensity aerobic PA. The National Institute for Health and Care Excellence recommends that adults increase their PA levels to reduce the risk of dementia (5), but does not recommend guidelines for the amount of exercise recommended for people with dementia.

Previous research has suggested increased levels of PA as an intervention for maintaining cognitive performance in older adults (6, 7); for reviews, see Ref. (8, 9). Results of a meta-analysis of 29 randomized control trials (RCTs) found that sustained PA was associated with improved scores in memory, attention, and processing speed in both healthy adults and those with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (10). Similarly, a recent cross-sectional study of 7,000 older adults found that higher levels of objectively measured PA were associated with better performance in memory and executive function and lower prevalence of cognitive impairment (11). However, not all research has supported this association. For example, Young et al. (12) reviewed 12 RCTs of 750 participants and found that aerobic PA to have no cognitive benefit in cognitively healthy older adults.

The effect of PA has also been investigated in people with dementia, with a substantial body of research reporting a positive association between increased levels of PA and improved cognition [for reviews, see Ref. (1316)]. A RCT of 185 older adults with dementia found a significant effect of PF on dual-task performance (e.g., 10-m walking while counting backwards from 50 or naming the months backwards) and other tests of cognition [e.g., Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), Symbol Digit Modalities Test, the Stroop Colour and Word Test, and Lexical Verbal Fluency Test] (17). However, not all research has supported this positive effect. Forbes et al. (18) found no significant impact of exercise programs on improving cognitive function in a review of 17 RCTs studying 1,067 older adults with dementia, but did notice improvements in activities of daily living. Beyond improving outcomes for people with dementia, a systematic review of 26 studies reported a 14% reduction in dementia risk for those with high PA levels (RR 0.86, 95% CI 0.76–0.97) compared with those inactive (19), see also Ref. (2022).

The overall findings suggest an advantage of PA on cognitive ability even in individuals with dementia, but some of the present inconsistencies indicate a highly complex interaction between the level of PF, the type of PA, and the neuropsychological domains measured in various studies. In particular, studies found selective improvements in executive function rather than general cognitive benefits due to PA (2325). Therefore, it is recommended to use a battery of neuropsychological tests rather than a single measure of cognition (14, 26). For example, in a study that used multiple cognitive tests in oldest-old women (self-reported), PA was associated with better scores in the modified MMSE, category fluency, and Trails B, but not for digit span backwards, phonemic fluency, and delayed recall at 5 years follow-up (27). In another study, Matthews and Williams (28) found that Tai Chi was effective at improving performance in the Trails B and clock drawing tests, but not in the digit-symbol substitution test. Interactions between age and PA were also found for measures of immediate memory and visuospatial ability, and a trend for verbal ability (29).

Although most of the aforementioned work has focused on PA, there is less research addressing the role of PF in cognitive performance in dementia. A review of 16 intervention studies revealed improvements in physical functioning irrespective of the stage of dementia (30). For example, Santana-Sosa et al. (31) demonstrated improved PF after a 12-week exercise intervention period, but they did not assess cognitive function. However, Huh and colleagues (32) observed a cross-sectional association between executive functioning and PF, regardless of PA level. As of yet, there are no studies looking at both PA and PF on cognitive performance in dementia. Given the importance of ascertaining the potential differential contributions of PA and PF, more research is necessary to evaluate these specific associations with cognitive functioning, particularly among people with dementia.

Aims and Hypotheses

This study aimed to investigate the effect of PF on multiple cognitive outcomes, using a large battery of tests to provide a robust estimation of global cognitive functioning in individuals with dementia and healthy older adults. The areas investigated were general cognitive ability, recognition and recall memory, verbal fluency, visual memory, prospective memory (PM), and retrospective memory (RM). It was hypothesized that those with high levels of PF would have better cognitive performance, irrespective of their health status. The secondary aim of the research was to investigate the differential impact of current PF levels on specific cognitive measures. It was hypothesized that the effect of PA would differ across distinct cognitive domains.

Materials and Methods

Design

A cross-sectional case–control method was used to administer a battery of five standardized cognitive tests to a group of people with dementia and a group of controls aged 65 and older. Current levels of PF were assessed using the Physical Fitness Questionnaire (PFQ). Participants also answered demographic and lifestyle questions (for individuals with dementia, most of this information were provided by their family member or carer accompanying them).

Participants

A total of 70 participants took part in the study (30 with dementia and 40 healthy controls). Inclusion criteria for all participants were age of 65+; dementia participants had to have received an official medical diagnosis, and healthy participants had to be in good health (e.g., no depression or other psychiatric conditions that could affect cognitive performance). Dementia participants were recruited through advertisement via “Join Dementia Research” website, as well as at Alzheimer Cafes, which is a monthly social gathering for individuals with dementia and their carers organized by the Alzheimer Café UK Charity across various locations in London and Surrey. Healthy participants were recruited through the use of advertisement posters, word of mouth and local church and community groups. UCL Ethics Committee granted ethical approval for the study. Each participant provided written informed consent.

Tests and Procedure

Participants completed the following tests.

The MMSE (33)

An assessment of overall cognitive performance. Scores ranged from 0 to 30 with higher scores indicating better performance.

The California Verbal Learning Test [CVLT-9 (34)]

A shorter nine-word version of the CVLT-II (35) was used, designed specifically for people with dementia, which provided five different outcomes of interest: list A learning trials 1–5 (CVLT immediate recall), total errors including intrusions and repetitions during recall (CVLT recall errors), recall of words freely (CVLT free recall), recall of words prompted by semantic categories (CVLT cued recall), correct identification of 9 words in a list containing 24 interruption words (CVLT recognition accuracy). Scores for CVLT immediate recall range from 0 to 36 and scores for CVLT free and cued recall ranged from 0 to 9 with higher scores indicating better performance. From the number of correct hits and false positives, a d-prime score was calculated as an unbiased measure of recognition accuracy, with higher scores indicating better accuracy. A higher score for CVLT recall errors indicated more errors during the recall.

The Verbal Fluency Test [VF (36)]

The VF test provided a measure of verbal, language, and semantic ability as well as executive control (37). Participants were asked to name as many animals as possible in a 2-min period. A score of 1 was given for each correct animal recalled. Higher scores indicated better performance.

The Clock Drawing Test [CD (38)]

Participants were asked to draw a clock with the time reading 10 past 11, providing a measure of visual memory. Scores ranged from 0 to 6, a score of 0 indicating no resemblance to a clock, and 6 indicating a perfect clock, with all numbers in the correct place and the correct time given. Results were interpreted according to Shulman’s six-point scoring system (39).

The Prospective and Retrospective Memory Questionnaire [PRMQ (40)]

The questionnaire contained eight items of PM and eight items of RM in the context of everyday life. Independent scores were calculated for PM and RM, ranging from 8 to 40 for each questionnaire, with higher scores indicating lower prospective or RM.

Physical Fitness Questionnaire (41)

Participants also completed the PFQ (41), a 15-item questionnaire specially designed for older adults to assess their risk for dementia. Questions address aerobic conditioning, strength, and balance or stability. A cutoff score of 33 was used to group participants into “high” or “low” PF. Scores ranged from 15 to 75, with lower scores indicating better overall PF.

Tests were administered face to face by the researcher in the participant’s home or at an Alzheimer Café. A carer or family member was present to help provide information about the individuals with dementia (such as a history of head injury, or midlife levels of PA). Once inclusion criteria had been met, and written consent had been given, participants answered some questions about their demographics and lifestyle information. They then completed the 15 questions of the PFQ, after which they were guided by the researcher through a battery of five standardized cognitive tests, in the order as presented earlier. The CVLT-9 covered both short- and long-term memory and were administered separately. The VF, CLOCK DRAWING, and PRMQ were completed after the CVLT short-term memory questions, to introduce a temporary delay before the long-term memory questions. The total duration of testing was approximately 1 h.

The lifestyle and demographics questionnaire included questions related to participants’ age, gender, education, any history of head injuries which resulted in hospitalization, previous levels of exercise during midlife (low, moderate, or vigorous levels of PA), and the date of dementia diagnosis (for the dementia group only).

Statistical Analyses

The demographic information was used to select covariates among age, gender, education, occupation, head injury, and previous levels of exercise in the analyses. A 2 × 2 between-subject multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) analyses was used to explore the effect of PF and health status (healthy or dementia) on 10 different test measures assessing cognitive performance. Age and previous levels of exercise were included in the models as covariates, as both revealed univariate associations with the dependent measures (see Table S1 in Supplementary Material). Discriminant analysis was used to assess the observed multivariate interaction in the MANCOVA. All analyses were conducted using SPSS version 24.

Results

Table 1 presents descriptive statistics for the 70 participants grouped according to their health status (healthy and dementia) and PF level. Healthy controls had significantly higher levels of current PF than the dementia group. Individuals with dementia were 5.4 years older than the healthy group. There were more women with low PF compared to men. Interestingly, high levels of past exercise were associated with high PF but were not related to health status.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographics of sample.

Table 2 presents the means and SDs of the test scores separated by PF level and health status.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive and inferential statistics for the univariate analyses.

A MANCOVA was conducted to assess the effects of PF and health status on the dependent measures, including as covariates age and previous levels of exercise, as these were the only demographic variables that showed at least one significant univariate test result (see Table S1 in Supplementary Material). The pattern of results remained the same when these covariates were excluded from the analysis.

There were significant multivariate effects of health status [Pillai’s Trace1 = 0.68, F(10, 55) = 11.58, p < 0.001, observed power = 1.00] and level of PF [Pillai’s Trace = 0.31, F(10, 55) = 2.47, p < 0.05, observed power = 0.91], and an interaction between these factors [Pillai’s Trace = 0.28, F(10, 55) = 2.13, p < 0.05, observed power = 0.85]. To investigate these effects, discriminant analysis (method = enter) was used in which the combined set of 10 measures predicted the health status by the level of PF.

The overall test was significant (Wilks λ = 0.15, χ2 = 119.42, df = 30, canonical correlation = 0.87, R2 = 0.75, p < 0.001). Reclassification of cases based on the canonical variables was successful: 71.4% of the cases were correctly reclassified into their original categories, but 54.3% of the cross-validated cases were correctly classified. The misclassifications were predominantly due to dementia cases with high PF being misclassified as healthy (45% compared to 16% for low PF dementia cases), whereas the reverse misclassification was less likely (7% for low PF healthy and 4% high PF healthy).

The analysis extracted two functions (eigenvalues: 3.045 and 0.455), accounting for 95.4% of the variance in cognitive performance, with the first function accounting for 83%. This function was labeled “Global cognition,” as for all dependent measures, better scores increased the value from the discriminant function. Figure 1 shows the resulting values from function 1 against the reference of the minimal value (=−4.14) in the sample. The interaction observed from the MANCOVA reflects better global cognition for dementia cases who are physically fit compared to those who are not, combined with a lack of such an effect for the healthy controls. Using fitted values for each individual, a two-way ANOVA crossing health status and PF revealed main effects of status [F(1, 66) = 145.42, p < 0.001, partial η2 =0.69, observed power = 1.00] and PF [F(1, 66) = 11.69, p = 0.001, partial η2 = 0.15, observed power = 0.92] and an interaction [F(1, 66) = 8.58, p < 0.01, partial η2 = 0.12, observed power = 0.82], due to a significant simple effect of PF for dementia cases [t(28) = 3.18, p < 0.01], but not for healthy controls (p > 0.90). These results remained significant when including age, past exercise, and time since diagnosis (for the dementia group) as covariates.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Global cognitive function score as a function of health status and level of physical fitness. The scores were estimated using the first discriminant function, which accounts for 83% of the variance. All scores were referenced to the minimal value of -4.14. Error bars represent standard errors from the mean (SEM).

Whereas the above two-way ANOVA takes into account all dependent measures in a linear-weighted manner, univariate analyses on each measure (corrected for multiple testing) lack the necessary univariate power. The pattern of results differed across dependent measures (see Table 2). All dependent measures revealed better performance for healthy individuals compared to dementia patients (all p < 0.001). Only MMSE and clock drawing, exhibited better performance for high compared to low PF. There were interaction effects just for clock drawing, with larger effects of PF for those with dementia in contrast to the control group when taking into account age, past exercise, and years since diagnosis as covariates.

Discussion

This study explored the influence of PF on cognitive functioning in participants with dementia and healthy controls. As expected, those with dementia showed lower levels of cognitive functioning than healthy group. Of interest are the findings that current PF levels were found to have a significant overall effect on cognition specifically on MMSE and clock drawing.

The association between PF and MMSE is an interesting finding, given that previous research into the effects of PA on MMSE in people with dementia and MCI has provided inconsistent results. For example, whereas some RCTs (4245) show positive effects of PA on MMSE, Venturelli et al. (46) did not find a strong advantage for their walking intervention. However, the decline in MMSE scores was lower in the intervention group compared to the control group, suggesting a relative attenuation of decline with exercise, although the interaction did not reach significance after correcting for multiple testing.

Performance on the clock drawing task was significantly affected by PF levels in dementia participants but not in control participants. The abilities required for clock drawing depends on executive functions, such as visuoconstructive abilities, spatial function, planning, abstract thinking, and attention (47). This research supports the hypothesis proposed by Kramer et al. (24) that executive control processes showed the most considerable benefit of improved fitness [see also Ref. (23, 25, 48)].

Only clock drawing revealed a reliable interaction between health status and PF, highlighting different sensitivities of the tests to the combination of health status and PF level. The reason for this may be related to the requirement of visual processing which has been shown to be particularly sensitive to dementia-related cognitive decline [e.g., Ref. (49)].

With regard to the demographic information, although high levels of past exercise were associated with high PF, it was not related to health status. This suggests that high levels of PA may not in and of itself reduce the incidence of dementia, but when dementia does occur a history of high levels of physical exercise may limit the amount (or perhaps the rate) of cognitive deterioration. With a cross-sectional design, this study is unable to address this pattern of decline in more detail but provides a direction for longitudinal analyses.

Potential Biological Mechanisms

Experimental and cohort studies demonstrate that long-term regular PA alters brain structure and cognitive function (5052).

The differential profile of positive effects on cognitive functioning suggests that the impact of PA is more complex than the argument that PA positively affects cognitive function by enhancing general brain vitality (15). A possible explanation for this could be that PA stimulates blood circulation in frontostriatal circuits, improving executive function in older adults with dementia. Some support for this comes from the finding that PA is associated with angiogenesis and enhanced blood flow in brain regions that are involved in executive functioning (53).

Evidence from animal studies suggests that aerobic exercise increases the levels of growth factors (e.g., BDNF), capillary blood supply to the prefrontal cortex, the growth of new neurons, and as a result enhanced cognitive performance (54, 55). In humans, Colcombe and Kramer (23) reported that 6 months aerobic walking significantly increased activity in the prefrontal cortical areas and enhanced executive function task performance. Furthermore, in a recent prospective study, Tan et al. (56) observed a positive linear association between (self-reported) PA and total brain and hippocampal volumes together with a reduced risk of dementia.

Functional imaging studies that focus on PF found greater hippocampal volumes and denser striatal structures that predicted performance in executive tasks (57, 58). Although this work investigated children, it demonstrates that PF alone is associated with structural brain changes. Manckoundia and colleagues (59) showed that the spatiotemporal dynamics of sit-to-stand and back-to-sit, which are movements involved in the 30-s chair stand test differ between healthy adults and those with dementia. Auyeung et al. (60) demonstrated that the association between PF and cognitive functioning was independent of muscle strength. These findings are interpreted as the neural integrity influences both tests of PF (including strength and balance) and cognitive functioning.

Strengths and Limitations

In this cross-sectional study, we investigated the associations between PF and cognitive performance. The use of self-report measures of PF represents a potential limitation of this study since we did not have other objective measures available. However, we placed considerable effort in accurately estimating current levels of PF and past levels of PA of the participants with dementia through arranging interviews with family members who knew the individual for a more extended period of time. Furthermore, there is evidence from several studies suggesting that the self-report measures of physical ability tend to be highly correlated with objective test scores (61, 62). Finally, in a recent systematic review, Stephen et al. (63) observed that all 24 prospective and intervention studies they considered used self-report with most (18 out of 24) studies showing a reduced risk of Alzheimer’s disease. Also, the PFQ asks about balance, strength, and aerobic, which bare similarity to ADL, which Forbes et al. (18) showed to be positively associated with objective measures of PA. Future studies could expand the range of metrics used to assess PF, including tests such as the timed up and go test (64) and the 30-s chair stand test (65), to evaluate its link with cognition in healthy people and those with dementia.

This research used an extensive battery of memory tests, each of which has been widely used and validated in dementia research. This is highly useful for providing a robust assessment of cognition and memory, despite being costly in time for the participants and researchers. By using a battery of multiple cognitive tests, a latent construct of global cognitive functioning could be extracted that weights each test accordingly to maximize its classification accuracy. This approach takes into account the differential sensitivity of tests to global cognitive ability. Using a much-extended battery of tests that covers a wider range of cognitive domains would be desirable to address latent constructs of each domain separately.

Conclusion

Physical fitness is associated with higher global cognition in individuals with dementia, independent of their levels of past exercise and illness duration. This research finds significant positive effects of PF on some, but not all cognitive tests in individuals with dementia. Results from this study support the WHO recommendation for increased PF for older adults in general and those with dementia in particular. Future research should focus on how this can be made accessible to older adults who may have mobility issues, physical disabilities, and cognitive impairment.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the UCL Research Ethics Committee with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the UCL Research Ethics Committee (Project ID Number: 6439/003).

Author Contributions

DC and AH designed the study and collected the data. DC, AH, and ED analyzed the data and wrote the paper.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The reviewer LV and handling editor declared their shared affiliation.

Acknowledgments

The researchers would like to thank “Join Dementia Research”, “Alzheimer Café UK” and in particular to Kate Little from “Memories Cafe – Age UK” for their tremendous help and assistance in recruiting participants for this study. This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Funding

DC is supported by the National Institute on Aging (NIA) (Grant 528182). The content of this work is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the NIA.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at http://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpubh.2017.00346/full#supplementary-material.

Footnote

  1. ^Pillai’s trace values are reported as the Box’s M test was significant [M = 513.12, F(165, 4,960) = 2.08, p < 0.001].

References

1. Department of Health. (2009). Available from: http://www.wired-gov.net/wg/wg-news-1.nsf/lfi/408275

Google Scholar

2. Sevigny J, Chiao P, Bussière T, Weinreb PH, Williams L, Maier M, et al. The antibody aducanumab reduces Aβ plaques in Alzheimer’s disease. Nature (2016) 537(7618):50–6. doi:10.1038/nature19323

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Cyarto EV, Lautenschlager NT, Desmond PM, Ames D, Szoeke C, Salvado O, et al. Protocol for a randomized controlled trial evaluating the effect of physical activity on delaying the progression of white matter changes on MRI in older adults with memory complaints and mild cognitive impairment: the AIBL Active trial. BMC Psychiatry (2012) 12:167. doi:10.1186/1471-244X-12-167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. World Health Organization. Global Strategy on Diet, Physical Activity & Health. WHO (2016).

Google Scholar

5. NICE Guidelines. Dementia, Disability and Frailty in Later Life – Mid-life Approaches to Delay or Prevent Onset. (2015). Available from: https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ng16

Google Scholar

6. Lautenschlager NT, Cox KL, Flicker L, Foster JK, van Bockxmeer FM, Xiao J, et al. Effect of physical activity on cognitive function in older adults at risk for Alzheimer disease: a randomized trial. JAMA (2008) 300(9):1027–37. doi:10.1001/jama.300.9.1027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Matei R, Thune-Boyle I, Hamer M, Lliffe S, Fox KR, Jefferis B. Acceptability of a theory-based sedentary behaviour reduction intervention for older adults (‘On Your Feet to Earn Your Seat’). BMC Public Health (2015) 15:606. doi:10.1186/s12889-015-1921-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Angevaren M, Aufdemkampe G, Verhaar HJ, Aleman A, Vanhees L. Physical activity and enhanced fitness to improve cognitive function in older people without known cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database Syst Rev (2008) 3:CD005381. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD005381.pub3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Sofi F, Valecchi D, Bacci D, Abbate R, Gensini GF, Casini A, et al. Physical activity and risk of cognitive decline: a meta-analysis of prospective studies. J Intern Med (2011) 269(1):107–17. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2796.2010.02281.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Smith PJ, Blumenthal JA, Hoffman BM, Cooper H, Strauman TA, Welsh-Bohmer K, et al. Aerobic exercise and neurocognitive performance: a meta-analytic review of randomized controlled trials. Psychosom Med (2010) 72(3):239–52. doi:10.1097/PSY.0b013e3181d14633

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Zhu W, Howard VJ, Wadley VG, Hutto B, Blair SN, Vena JE, et al. Association between objectively measured physical activity and cognitive function in older adults – the reasons for geographic and racial differences in stroke study. J Am Geriatr Soc (2015) 63(12):2447–54. doi:10.1111/jgs.13829

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Young J, Angevaren M, Rusted J, Tabet N. Aerobic exercise to improve cognitive function in older people without known cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database Syst Rev (2015) 4:CD005381. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD005381.pub4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Kirk-Sanchez NJ, McGough EL. Physical exercise and cognitive performance in the elderly: current perspectives. Clin Interv Aging (2014) 9:51–62. doi:10.2147/CIA.S39506

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Farina N, Rusted J, Tabet N. The effect of exercise interventions on cognitive outcome in Alzheimer’s disease: a systematic review. Int Psychogeriatr (2014) 26(1):9–18. doi:10.1017/S1041610213001385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Groot C, Hooghiemstra AM, Raijmakers PGHM, van Berckel BNM, Scheltens P, Scherder EJA, et al. The effect of physical activity on cognitive function in patients with dementia: a meta-analysis of randomized control trials. Ageing Res Rev (2016) 25:13–23. doi:10.1016/j.arr.2015.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Falck RS, Davis JC, Liu-Ambrose T. What is the association between sedentary behaviour and cognitive function? A systematic review. Br J Sports Med (2016) 51(10):800–11. doi:10.1136/bjsports-2015-095551

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Sobol NA, Hoffmann K, Vogel A, Lolk A, Gottrup H, Høgh P, et al. Associations between physical function, dual-task performance and cognition in patients with mild Alzheimer’s disease. Aging Ment Health (2016) 20(11):1139–46. doi:10.1080/13607863.2015.1063108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Forbes D, Thiessen EJ, Blake CM, Forbes SC, Forbes S. Exercise programs for people with dementia. Cochrane Database Syst Rev (2015) 4:CD006489. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD006489.pub4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Blondell SJ, Hammersley-Mather R, Veerman JL. Does physical activity prevent cognitive decline and dementia? A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. BMC Public Health (2014) 14:510. doi:10.1186/1471-2458-14-510

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Norton S, Mathews FE, Barnes DE, Yaffe K, Brayne C. Potential for primary prevention of Alzheimer’s disease: an analysis of population-based data. Lancet Neurol (2014) 13(8):788–94. doi:10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70136-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Beydoun MA, Beydoun HA, Gamaldo AA, Teel A, Zonderman AB, Wang Y. Epidemiologic studies of modifiable factors associated with cognition and dementia: systematic review and meta-analysis. BMC Public Health (2014) 14:643. doi:10.1186/1471-2458-14-643

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Beckett MW, Ardern CI, Rotondi MA. A meta-analysis of prospective studies on the role of physical activity and the prevention of Alzheimer’s disease in older adults. BMC Geriatr (2015) 15:9. doi:10.1186/s12877-015-0007-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Colcombe S, Kramer AF. Fitness effects on the cognitive function of older adults a meta-analytic study. Psychol Sci (2003) 14(2):125–30. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.t01-1-01430

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Kramer AF, Hahn S, Cohen NJ, Banich MT, McAuley E, Harrison CR, et al. Ageing, fitness and neurocognitive function. Nature (1999) 400(6743):418–9. doi:10.1038/22682

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Smiley-Oyen AL, Lowry KA, Francois SJ, Kohut ML, Ekkekakis P. Exercise, fitness, and neurocognitive function in older adults: the “selective improvement” and “cardiovascular fitness” hypotheses. Ann Behav Med (2008) 36(3):280–91. doi:10.1007/s12160-008-9064-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Yu F. Guiding research and practice: a conceptual model for aerobic exercise training in Alzheimer’s disease. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen (2011) 26(3):184–94. doi:10.1177/1533317511402317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Wang S, Luo X, Barnes D, Sano M, Yaffe K. Physical activity and risk of cognitive impairment among oldest-old women. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry (2014) 22(11):1149–57. doi:10.1016/j.jagp.2013.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Matthews MM, Williams HG. Can Tai chi enhance cognitive vitality? A preliminary study of cognitive executive control in older adults after A Tai chi intervention. J S C Med Assoc (2008) 104(8):255–7.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

29. Okonkwo OC, Schultz SA, Oh JM, Larson J, Edwards D, Cook D, et al. Physical activity attenuates age-related biomarker alterations in preclinical AD. Neurology (2014) 83(19):1753–60. doi:10.1212/WNL.0000000000000964

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Blankevoort CG, van Heuvelen MJG, Boersma F, Luning H, de Jong J, Scherder EJA. Review of effects of physical activity on strength, balance, mobility and ADL performance in elderly subjects with dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord (2010) 30(5):392–402. doi:10.1159/000321357

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Santana-Sosa E, Barriopedro MI, López-Mojares LM, Pérez M, Lucia A. Exercise training is beneficial for Alzheimer’s patients. Int J Sports Med (2008) 29(10):845–50. doi:10.1055/s-2008-1038432

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Huh Y, Yang EJ, Lee SA, Lim J-Y, Kim KW, Paik N-J. Association between executive function and physical performance in older Korean adults: findings from the Korean Longitudinal Study on Health and Aging (KLoSHA). Arch Gerontol Geriatr (2011) 52(3):e156–61. doi:10.1016/j.archger.2010.10.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res (1975) 12(3):189–98. doi:10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Libon DJ, Mattson RE, Glosser G, Kaplan E, Malamut BL, Sands LP, et al. A nine-word dementia version of the California Verbal Learning test. Clin Neuropsychol (1996) 10(3):237–44. doi:10.1080/13854049608406686

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Delis DC, Kramer JH, Kaplan E, Ober BA. Manual for the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT-II). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation (2000).

Google Scholar

36. Ratcliff G, Ganguli M, Chandra V, Sharma S, Belle S, Seaberg E, et al. Effects of literacy and education on measures of word fluency. Brain Lang (1998) 61(1):115–22. doi:10.1006/brln.1997.1858

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Fisk JE, Sharp CA. Age-related impairment in executive functioning: updating, inhibition, shifting, and access. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol (2004) 26(7):874–90. doi:10.1080/13803390490510680

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Freedman M, Leach L, Kaplan E, Winocur G, Shulman KI, Delis DC. Clock Drawing: A Neuropsychological Analysis. New York: Oxford University Press (1994).

Google Scholar

39. Shulman KI, Gold DP, Cohen CA, Zucchero CA. Clock-drawing and dementia in the community: a longitudinal study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry (1993) 8(6):487–96. doi:10.1002/gps.930080606

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Smith G, Della Sala S, Logie RH, Maylor EA. Prospective and retrospective memory in normal ageing and dementia: a questionnaire study. Memory (2000) 8(5):311–21. doi:10.1080/09658210050117735

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Small G, Vorgan G. The Alzheimer’s Prevention Program: Keep Your Brain Healthy for the Rest of Your Life. New York: Workman Publishing (2012).

Google Scholar

42. Van de Winckel A, Feys H, De Weerdt W, Dom R. Cognitive and behavioural effects of music-based exercises in patients with dementia. Clin Rehabil (2004) 18(3):253–60. doi:10.1191/0269215504cr750oa

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Baum EE, Jarjoura D, Polen AE, Faur D, Rutecki G. Effectiveness of a group exercise program in a long-term care facility: a randomized pilot trial. J Am Med Dir Assoc (2003) 4(2):74–80. doi:10.1097/01.JAM.0000053513.24044.6C

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Suzuki T, Shimada H, Makizako H, Doi T, Yoshida D, Ito K, et al. A randomized controlled trial of multicomponent exercise in older adults with mild cognitive impairment. PLoS One (2013) 8(4):e61483. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0061483

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Kwak YS, Um SY, Son TG, Kim DJ. Effect of regular exercise on senile dementia patients. Int J Sports Med (2008) 29(6):471–4. doi:10.1055/s-2007-964853

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Venturelli M, Scarsini R, Schena F. Six-month walking program changes cognitive and ADL performance in patients with Alzheimer. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen (2011) 26(5):381–8. doi:10.1177/1533317511418956

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Agrell B, Dehlin O. The clock-drawing test. Age Ageing (1998) 27:399–403. doi:10.1093/ageing/27.3.399

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Hall CD, Smith AL, Keele SW. The impact of aerobic activity on cognitive function in older adults: a new synthesis based on the concept of executive control. Eur J Cogn Psychol (2001) 13(1–2):279–300. doi:10.1080/09541440126012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Didic M, Felician O, Barbeau EJ, Mancini J, Latger-Florence C, Tramoni E, et al. Impaired visual recognition memory predicts Alzheimer’s disease in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord (2013) 35(5–6):291–9. doi:10.1159/000347203

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Colcombe SJ, Kramer AF, Erickson KI, Scalf P, McAuley E, Cohen NJ, et al. Cardiovascular fitness, cortical plasticity, and aging. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2004) 101(9):3316–21. doi:10.1073/pnas.0400266101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Colcombe SJ, Erickson KI, Scalf PE, Kim JS, Prakash R, McAuley E, et al. Aerobic exercise training increases brain volume in aging humans. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci (2006) 61(11):1166–70. doi:10.1093/gerona/61.11.1166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Weuve J, Kang JH, Manson JE, Breteler MM, Ware JH, Grodstein F. Physical activity, including walking, and cognitive function in older women. JAMA (2004) 292(12):1454–61. doi:10.1001/jama.292.12.1454

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Erickson KI, Miller DL, Weinstein AM, Akl SL, Banducci SE. Physical activity and brain plasticity in late adulthood: a conceptual and comprehensive review. Ageing Res (2012) 3(1):34–47. doi:10.4081/ar.2012.e6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Gobbo OL, O’Mara SM. Exercise, but not environmental enrichment, improves learning after kainic acid-induced hippocampal neurodegeneration in association with an increase in brain-derived neurotrophic factor. Behav Brain Res (2005) 159(1):21–6. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2004.09.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Neeper SA, Gomez-Pinilla F, Choi J, Cotman CW. Physical activity increases mRNA for brain-derived neurotrophic factor and nerve growth factor in rat brain. Brain Res (1996) 726(1–2):49–56. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(96)00273-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Tan ZS, Spartano NL, Beiser AS, DeCarli C, Auerbach SH, Vasan RS, et al. Physical activity, brain volume, and dementia risk: the Framingham study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci (2017) 72(6):789–95. doi:10.1093/gerona/glw130

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Chaddock L, Erickson KI, Prakash RS, Kim JS, Voss MW, Vanpatter M, et al. A neuroimaging investigation of the association between aerobic fitness, hippocampal volume, and memory performance in preadolescent children. Brain Res (2010) 1358:172–83. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2010.08.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Chaddock L, Erickson KI, Prakash RS, Voss MW, VanPatter M, Pontifex MB, et al. A functional MRI investigation of the association between childhood aerobic fitness and neurocognitive control. Biol Psychol (2012) 89(1):260–8. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.10.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Manckoundia P, Mourey F, Pfitzenmeyer P, Papaxanthis C. Comparison of motor strategies in sit-to-stand and back-to-sit motions between healthy and Alzheimer’s disease elderly subjects. Neuroscience (2006) 137(2):385–92. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2005.08.079

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Auyeung TW, Kwok T, Lee J, Leung PC, Leung J, Woo J. Functional decline in cognitive impairment – the relationship between physical and cognitive function. Neuroepidemiology (2008) 31(3):167–73. doi:10.1159/000154929

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Daly M, McMinn D, Allan JL. A bidirectional relationship between physical activity and executive function in older adults. Front Hum Neurosci (2015) 8:1044. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.01044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Buchman AS, Boyle PA, Yu L, Shah RC, Wilson RS, Bennett DA. Total daily physical activity and the risk of AD and cognitive decline in older adults. Neurology (2012) 78(17):1323–9. doi:10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182535d35

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Stephen R, Hongisto K, Solomon A, Lönnroos E. Physical activity and Alzheimer’s disease: a systematic review. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci (2017) 72(6):733–9. doi:10.1093/gerona/glw251

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Podsiadlo D, Richardson S. The timed ‘Up & Go’: a test of basic functional mobility for frail elderly persons. J Am Geriatr Soc (1991) 39(2):142–8. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.1991.tb01616.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Jones CJ, Rikli RE, Beam WC. A 30-s chair-stand test as a measure of lower body strength in community-residing older adults. Res Q Exerc Sport (1999) 70(2):113–9. doi:10.1080/02701367.1999.10608028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: dementia, aging, physical activity, physical fitness, cognition

Citation: Hollamby A, Davelaar EJ and Cadar D (2017) Increased Physical Fitness Is Associated with Higher Executive Functioning in People with Dementia. Front. Public Health 5:346. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2017.00346

Received: 25 June 2017; Accepted: 04 December 2017;
Published: 21 December 2017

Edited by:

Alexandre Andrade Loch, University of São Paulo, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Leandro Da Costa Lane Valiengo, University of São Paulo, Brazil
Andreas Hoell, Zentralinstitut für Seelische Gesundheit, Germany
Pawel Izdebski, Kazimierz Wielki University in Bydgoszcz, Poland

Copyright: © 2017 Hollamby, Davelaar and Cadar. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Dorina Cadar, d.cadar@ucl.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.