Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 24 September 2013
Sec. Developmental Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic The development of the understanding of emotional expressions View all 5 articles

Spanish parents' emotion talk and their children's understanding of emotion

  • 1Faculty of Arts and Social Sciences, Kingston University, London, UK
  • 2School of Psychology, University of Surrey, Guildford, UK

Relations between parent-child emotion talk and children's emotion understanding were examined in 63 Spanish mothers and fathers and their 4- (M = 53.35 months, SD = 3.86) and 6-year-old (M = 76.62 months, SD = 3.91) children. Parent-child emotion talk was analyzed during two storytelling tasks: a play-related storytelling task and a reminiscence task (conversation about past experiences). Children's emotion understanding was assessed twice through a standardized test of emotion comprehension (TEC; Pons et al., 2004), once before one of the two parent-child storytelling sessions and again 6 months later. Mothers' use of emotion labels during the play-related storytelling task predicted children's emotion understanding after controlling for children's previous emotion understanding. Whereas fathers' use of emotion labels during the play-related storytelling task was correlated with children's emotion understanding, it did not predict children's emotion understanding after controlling for previous emotion understanding. Implications of these findings for future research on children's socioemotional development are discussed.

Spanish Parent-Child Emotion Talk and Children's Understanding of Emotions

Emotion understanding is the ability to recognize, label, interpret, and respond to our own and others' emotions. Children's emotion understanding has been found to be an early predictor of later social adaptation (Izard et al., 2001), children's peer acceptance and popularity, prosocial behavior, and emotion regulation skills (Denham et al., 1990; Cassidy et al., 1992; Garner et al., 1994; Garner and Power, 1996; Hoffman, 2000). Children who have difficulty understanding and regulating their emotions have problems in their social relations with others (Denham et al., 1990; Kim and Cicchetti, 2010) and are likely to display long-term behavioral problems (Rydell et al., 2003). Children's understanding of emotions has also been linked to academic performance (Izard et al., 2001; Garner, 2010; Valiente et al., 2012), as well as to the development of psychopathology (Cicchetti et al., 1995; Kring and Bachorowski, 1999; Suveg et al., 2011).

Three broad levels of children's cognitive understanding of emotions have been identified (Pons et al., 2004). First, when children are 3 years of age they understand external aspects of emotion such as its situational causes (e.g., different situations provoke individuals to experience different emotions), its outward expression (e.g., individuals tend to express their emotions), and reminders' effect on affect (e.g., when someone reminds a child about a recently deceased pet, the child might experience sadness again). Children typically master this level of understanding by five. Between 5 and 7 years of age, children's understanding of emotions changes from a situational understanding to a mentalistic understanding of emotions (Wellman et al., 2001). They learn to recognize hidden emotions (Harris et al., 1986; Joshi and MacLean, 1994), and to understand the relationship between desires, beliefs and emotions (Harris et al., 1989). Finally, when children are between 7 and 11 years of age, they can reflect on an emotion from different perspectives. Children can understand ambivalent emotions, identify moral emotions, and are able regulate their emotions cognitively (Pons et al., 2004).

Despite evidence for a developmental pattern in children's emotion understanding, there are individual differences in children's emotion understanding from a very early age (Pons and Harris, 2005). Factors that underlie individual differences in children's emotion understanding have been investigated. During early infancy, parent-child emotion talk (Denham et al., 2000) and the emotional climate in the family (Denham et al., 2007; Zahn-Waxler, 2010) have been found to influence children's emotion understanding. Parental emotional expressiveness, its intensity, and parents' reactions to their children's expression of emotions have also been identified as influencing factors on children's emotion understanding (Denham et al., 1994).

Special attention has been paid to mother-child emotion talk and its relation with children's emotion understanding. In general, research has focused on the frequency and type of emotion talk (Dunn et al., 1987; Martin and Green, 2005). Indeed, the degree to which a child understands emotions is influenced by the frequency that his or her mother talks to him or her about emotions (Dunn et al., 1987; Denham et al., 1994; Denham and Auerbach, 1995; Halberstadt et al., 1999; Harris et al., 2005; Laible and Song, 2006). For example, Dunn et al. (1991) concluded that 3-year-olds living in families where emotions were discussed more frequently obtained higher scores than their peers when judging someone else's emotions at age six. Finally, Denham (1997) found that teachers rated children whose parents talked about emotions frequently as cooperative, empathic, and prosocial.

Not only is the frequency of mother-child emotion talk important, its quality might also be related to children's emotion understanding (Fivush, 1998). Specifically, it has been suggested that emotion explanations (e.g., “I am sad because my dog died”) and labels (e.g., “I am sad”) may help children conceptualize emotions differently (Cervantes and Callanan, 1998; Garner et al., 2008). Generally speaking, maternal explanations predict emotion understanding (Denham et al., 1994; Wellman and Lagattuta, 2004). Perhaps explanations about emotions allow children to learn about causes and consequences of emotions.

Little is known about the relation between father-child emotion talk and children's understanding of emotions. One way to address this gap in our knowledge is to directly compare mothers and fathers. The Spanish culture is an ideal context because one might expect differences between mothers and fathers, unlike American or UK samples. There have been repeated calls for more research on fathers and children (Lamb, 2010) because increasing research suggests that fathers influence children's outcomes, such as academic achievement (Fletcher et al., 1995). The little research there is on fathers has tended to focus more descriptively on how fathers talk to children (Reese et al., 1996). More research is needed that specifically looks at the link between fathers' talk and children's socio-emotional development.

Despite these calls for more research, only one study (Denham et al., 2010) has investigated fathers' emotion talk and children's understanding of emotions. Denham et al. (2010) found that mothers and fathers of preschool children differed in their socialization of emotions. Specifically, mothers acted as preschoolers' emotional gatekeepers, whereas fathers acted as preschoolers' playmates. However, mothers' and fathers' emotion socialization was similar for preschool boys and girls. Thus, based on this study we hypothesized that there would be differences in emotion talk between mothers and fathers. There are important differences between the present research and Denham's. First, Denham et al. (2010) asked parents and children to complete a reminiscence task, but not a storytelling task. In contrast, the present study incorporated a reminiscence task as well as a storytelling task. The use of both tasks is informative in that mothers' and fathers' talk during different tasks influences children's emotion understanding (Laible, 2004). The present study focused on mothers' and fathers' use of emotion labels and explanations across two different storytelling tasks. We expected children whose parents mentioned a higher proportion of emotion labels to have a better understanding of emotions than children whose parents mentioned fewer emotion labels. Second, Denham's study was restricted to children in early childhood. In addition to including 4-year-old children, we included 6-year-old children who represent a transitional phase between early and middle childhood. Finally, Denham et al.'s study examined North American children, whereas the current study focused on Spanish children.

Research suggests that culture influences children's understanding of emotions (Dunsmore et al., 2009; Halberstadt and Lozada, 2011; Perez-Rivera and Dunsmore, 2011). According to the enculturation perspective, children learn to understand how others feel and think embedded in the particular culture in which they live (Bruner, 1990). Similarly, culture influences parents' beliefs systems about how to raise children and about how to socialize their children's emotions (Dibiase and Gunnoe, 2004). Thus, parents and children influence each other with cultural expectations dictating how and when it is acceptable to display emotions (Brody, 1999).

Spain is regarded as a traditional culture with highly differentiated gender roles. Indeed, although through recent decades a higher number of women have joined the work forces (in 2010, 41.6% of women in Spain worked outside their homes), women still are children's primary caregivers and take care of most of the domestic chores (Instituto de la Mujer, 2012). In sum, Spanish women (similar to many Southern European women) hold a nurturant role and are regarded as the emotional keepers of the family (Dibiase and Gunnoe, 2004). We hypothesized that if Spanish mothers and fathers hold different roles when raising their children, Spanish mothers and fathers might have a distinct influence in their children's understanding of emotions. Given the amount of time that Spanish mothers compared to fathers spend with their children, we expected that there would be stronger relations between mothers' than fathers' talk with children's emotion understanding.

The Present Study

The present study focused on 4- and 6-year-old children because most research examining parent-child emotion talk and children's understanding of emotions has analyzed children up to the age of four (Dunn et al., 1991; Wang, 2001; Wellman et al., 2001; Martin and Green, 2005; Denham et al., 2010), leaving little research of children's emotion understanding after the age of four. Of special relevance is the analysis of 6-year-olds given that this is a transitional age between preschool and middle childhood. Research on children's emotion understanding during middle childhood is needed as it has been argued that this capacity is further developed through this age (Flavell et al., 1993; De Rosnay and Hughes, 2006).

Parent-child emotion talk was analyzed across two storytelling tasks: a play-related storytelling task and a reminiscence task. Storytelling is relevant because it is a cultural activity in which emotions, societal norms, and values are embedded (Fivush, 1989). We chose the play-related storytelling task because research indicates that play is a valuable setting to prompt discussion about emotions between parents and children (Dunn et al., 1987; Cervantes and Callanan, 1998; Perez-Rivera and Dunsmore, 2011). During play parents may guide children's beliefs and ideas about emotions and children can improve their understanding of emotions (Fivush, 1993). Indeed, stories serve as an important cultural tool for expressing socio-cognitive understanding of emotions and beliefs (Fivush, 1989). Moreover, it might be easier for children to discuss someone else's emotions rather than their own because they distance themselves to focus on a cognitive understanding of emotion. It has also been suggested that fictional narratives are a more controlled form of discourse than personal narratives (Bamberg and Damrad-Frye, 1991).

The reminiscence task was used because when reminiscing, parents and children not only discuss specific details of what happened but they also tend to discuss how those events made them feel (Fivush et al., 2009). Moreover, reminiscing gives children an opportunity to put emotions into perspective, which is more difficult when discussing present emotions (Fivush, 1989; Laible, 2004). Children's ability to elaborate on their personal narratives has been linked to their understanding of emotions (Cutting and Dunn, 1999). This study is the first to investigate parent-child emotion talk across these two types of task and relate it to children's emotion understanding. Based on previous research, we expected children of mothers and fathers who mentioned a higher proportion of emotion words to have a better understanding of emotions after controlling for prior emotion understanding. In addition, we expected that relations between mothers' talk and children's emotion understanding would be greater than that of fathers' talk and children's emotion understanding based on gender differences in Spanish culture.

Methods

Participants

Sixty-three children (30 girls and 33 boys), aged 4 (17 girls M = 53.20 months, SD = 3.86; range = 48–60 months; 18 boys, M = 53.21, SD = 3.88; range = 48.10–59.50 months) and 6-years-old (13 girls M = 76.79 months, SD = 3.92; range = 72–83.50 months; 15 boys, M = 76.21, SD = 3.86; range = 72–83.50 months) participated with both of their parents (mothers' age was M = 36.30 years, SD = 2.88; range = 29–42 years; fathers' age was M = 40.60 years, SD = 4.42; range = 34–54 years). The average number of children per family was 2.76 (SD = 0.95). Of the child participants, 24 were firstborns and the rest were later-borns. ANOVA models revealed no relations between number of siblings and children's birth order with emotion understanding or parental emotion talk.

All families were Spanish with Spanish as their first language. All families were intact from predominantly middle-to upper-middle class socioeconomic status. Parents had attended university. Participants were recruited on a volunteer basis. All parent signed an informed consent form. This study was part of a larger investigation of the relation between parent-child emotion talk, parent-child touch, and children's emotion understanding.

Materials

Play-related storytelling task

A plastic house and six family figures which included a grandfather, a grandmother, a father, a mother, a son, a daughter and a dog were used for the play-related storytelling task. The house was divided into four rooms: a kitchen, a living room, a bedroom, and a bathroom.

Reminiscence task

To elicit talk about emotions, parent-child dyads were given four events typed individually on index cards. Each card contained one of the following sentences: “a visit to the zoo,” “a visit to the doctor,” “the first day of school,” and “a time that the child fell down.”

Test of emotion comprehension

A standardized Spanish version of the Test of Emotion Comprehension (TEC, Pons et al., 2004) was administered to the child participants. The TEC assesses emotion understanding of 3-to-11 year old children by presenting vignettes in which a gender-matched protagonist encounters simple to complex situations eliciting different emotional responses. After each vignette, the child is asked how the protagonist feels by choosing from four illustrations of faces representing different emotional states (happiness, sadness, fear, normal, or anger). Vignettes are organized in an increasing order of difficulty (Pons et al., 2004). The TEC asks the child to identify emotions in 9 different situations, namely: (1) represented by facial expressions, (2) caused by external situations, (3) involving situations in which emotions result from desires, (4) consequence of a character's false belief, (5) elicited by reminders, (6) when a character attempts to control an emotion, (7) hidden, (8) conflicting, and (9) stemming from self-restraint. To test for understanding of hidden emotions, the child is asked how a teased character feels although he/she is smiling. Understanding of conflicting emotions is tested by asking the child about how a character feels after receiving a new bicycle when the character has never ridden a bicycle and could fall.

The TEC was the chosen test to assess children's level of emotion understanding because it has been widely used and replicated. Its different components are scalable (index of consistency I = 0.676) and the scale is valid (Coefficient of reproducibility R = 0.904; Pons et al., 2002). In addition, the TEC is different from other tests of emotion comprehension in the simplicity of the language that it uses (Pons et al., 2003). This reduces the effect of language ability on the understanding of emotions. The TEC has a high test-retest correlation (r = 0.83) within a 3-month period (Pons et al., 2002) and a 13-month period (r = 0.68; Pons and Harris, 2005) of the TEC has been found. The TEC has been used with Spanish-speaking children (Jimenez et al., 2013).

Procedure

Parent-child dyads were interviewed in their own homes on 2 separate days. Parents were informed that we were interested in parent-child interactions. On a first visit, the mother or the father and the child completed the play-related storytelling task and the reminiscence task. Within a minimum of one day and a maximum of 7 days, the other parent and the child completed the same two storytelling tasks. These two tasks were counterbalanced. Parent order was also counterbalanced. ANOVA models conducted on parents' speech variables revealed no effects of either parent or task order. The length of time devoted to the storytelling tasks was determined by participants as it has been argued (e.g., Kuebli and Fivush, 1992; Cervantes and Callanan, 1998; Fivush et al., 2000) that in this manner emotions are used in a more naturalistic manner. Mothers' conversations lasted for an average of 18.18 min (SD = 7.44) and fathers' conversations lasted for an average of 21.63 min (SD = 6.97). These sessions were videotaped.

In the play-related storytelling task, the first author asked the parent and the child to use the figures and the house to create a story together. To elicit the story, parent-child dyads were orally provided with four events: (1) the parents leave their children to go on an overnight trip, (2) the child falls down and hurts himself, (3) the dog runs away, and (4) the parents return home. This task has been very useful in eliciting discussion about emotions, and it has been used in a number of studies (e.g., Bretherton et al., 1990; Oppenheim et al., 1993; Cervantes and Callanan, 1998; Martin and Green, 2005). Events 1, 2, and 4 were taken from the attachment story-completion task by Bretherton et al. (1990). The four events have important emotional themes for preschool children (Cervantes and Callanan, 1998).

The other task was a reminiscence task. The first author gave parent-child dyads four events typed in four index cards: (i) the child's first day in school, (ii) a visit to the doctor, (iii) a time that the child fell down, and (iv) a trip to the zoo. Participants discussed the events in the order that they chose. These four events were used because of two reasons. First, they all involve important events for preschool children. Second, research suggests that not only do children's conversations about their own emotions play an important role in children's socialization of emotions, they may enhance children's understanding of emotions (Fivush, 1989).

In addition, the TEC was administered to the child participants before one of the two parent-child storytelling sessions and again 6 months later. All children completed the second TEC within a week of the 6-month mark. The TEC was administered in a quiet room in the presence of their parents. Its administration typically lasted 10 min.

Transcription and Coding

Videotaped conversations were transcribed verbatim by the first author and a native Spanish research assistant. The first author (a native Spanish speaker) coded all transcripts and the second author (a fluent Spanish speaker) checked them for accuracy. Transcripts were coded for mothers' and fathers' (1) total number of utterances, (2) total number of emotion utterances, and (3) emotion labels vs. emotion explanations.

Total number of utterances

The number of utterances made by the mother and the father were recorded. An utterance was a message unit bound by its intonation (Hoff-Ginsberg, 1991). Excluded were unintelligible utterances and false starts (Pancsofar and Vernon-Feagans, 2006). The first author typed transcripts into utterances, which were checked by the second author.

Total number of emotion words

Emotion words were those referring to a specific affective state (e.g., angry, jealous), or process (e.g., to have fun, to be furious, Cervantes, 2002). Total number of emotion words was calculated by adding together the total number of times each emotion word was used. The total number of emotion words mentioned by the mother and the father throughout the conversations were identified.

Emotion labels versus emotion explanations

All emotion words were coded as to whether they occurred in a label or explanation. Labels were emotion words that made reference to an emotion or ask about an emotion without including a causal relationship (e.g., “I am very happy”). Explanations were emotion references that asked about an emotion or that make a statement about an emotion including a causal relationship (e.g., “I am very happy because my mum gave me a present”). Emotion words were also coded as explanations if there was a causal link (e.g., “I am very happy because my mum gave me a present”), a lexical causative (e.g., “My mum made me very happy when she gave me a present) or if there was no explicit causal link but the utterances were adjacent and were rated as semantically causal (e.g., “I am very happy. My mum gave me a present”). These criteria are based on Bloom and Capatides (1987) and have been used in previous studies (Cervantes and Callanan, 1998; Martin and Green, 2005).

Reliability

Intercoder reliability was established separately for each coding scheme. Each child participant had four transcripts: one for each task (play-related storytelling task and reminiscence task) with each of his or her parents. The first author coded all transcripts and the second author coded 46 transcripts (20% of the data set). Coding was conducted in Spanish. Coders were not blind to the research hypotheses. Reliability was achieved with a K of 0.80 for the total number of emotion words, and with a K of 0.91 for the total number of emotion labels and explanations.

Scoring of the TEC

Following the standard TEC scoring procedure, children received a point for each of the nine components answered correctly, with a highest possible score of nine and lowest possible score of zero. Overall performance on the test was computed for each child by summing his or her scores on each of the nine emotion components. Children received credit when they indicated the correct emotion. The first two components, namely recognition and external cause, included five test items. Children received a single point on each of these components if they were correct on at least four of the five items. The third component, desire, included two test questions. Children received a single point if they were correct on both test items. The remaining six components included one test question. Children received one point for each emotion component on which they succeeded. Overall scores could range from zero to one on each component and from zero to nine on the entire test. Situational, mentalistic, and reflective levels of emotion were each based on three test questions added together.

Results

Each child had four transcripts: mother-child play-related storytelling task, mother-child reminiscence task, father-child play-related storytelling task, and father-child reminiscing task. Only children who produced four transcripts were included in the analysis.

Data Analyses

First, we present descriptive statistics for parents' talk and children's TEC scores. Next, correlations between the variables of interest are presented. Finally, regressions predicting children's emotion understanding at the second time point are presented.

Previous studies have analyzed emotion talk as total frequencies or as proportions. In this study and similar to previous research (e.g., Garner, 2003; Sales et al., 2003; Curenton and Craig, 2011; Garrett-Peters et al., 2011; Brownell et al., 2013), emotion words were analyzed as proportions. By using proportions rather than total frequencies, participants' total amount of talk was controlled. Proportions were calculated as the total number of emotion utterances divided by the total number of utterances. These were calculated separately for each participant.

Descriptive Statistics

The majority of parents discussed emotions throughout both storytelling tasks even though they were not explicitly asked to discuss emotions. During the play-related storytelling task, mothers used a mean proportion of 0.32 emotion labels (SD = 0.30) and 0.21 explanations (SD = 0.22), while during the reminiscence task, mothers used a mean proportion of 0.98 emotion labels (SD = 0.76) and 0.12 explanations of (SD = 0.15).

During the play-related storytelling task, fathers used a mean proportion of 0.24 emotion labels (SD = 0.22), and 0.10 explanations (SD = 0.12). Finally, fathers used a mean proportion of 0.66 emotion labels (SD = 0.58), and 0.08 explanations (SD = 0.14) during the reminiscence task.

Descriptive analyses on TEC 1 and TEC 2

Across both age groups, the minimum score was two and the maximum score was eight. A 2 (Children's age: 4, 6) × 2 (Children's gender: girl, boy) analysis of variance (ANOVA) conducted on the first administration of the TEC 1 as a dependent variable revealed no significant effect of gender. As expected, it revealed a significant age effect with 6-year-old children scoring higher on the TEC (M = 6.53, SD = 0.99) than 4-year-old children (M = 4.48, SD = 1.44), F(1, 62) = 42.35, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.41. A 2 (Children's age: 4, 6) × 2 (Children's gender: girl, boy) analysis of variance (ANOVA) conducted on the situational level of TEC 1 as a dependent variable revealed no significant effect of gender. As expected, it revealed a significant age effect with 6-year-olds scoring higher on the situational level of TEC 1 (M = 2.54, SD = 0.58) than 4-year-olds (M = 2.00, SD = 0.94), F(1, 62) = 6.79, p = 0.01, η2 = 0.10. There was also a significant Age × Gender interaction effect, F(1, 62) = 6.91, p = 0.01, η2 = 0.11. There was no difference between girls and boys at age 4, F(1, 62) = 2.14, p = 0.15, whereas boys (M = 2.80, SD = 0.41) outperformed girls (M = 2.23, SD = 0.60) at age 6, F(1, 27) = 8.75, p = 0.007, η2 = 0.25.

A 2 (Children's age: 4, 6) × 2 (Children's gender: girl, boy) analysis of variance (ANOVA) conducted on the mentalistic level of TEC 1 as a dependent variable revealed no significant effect of gender. As expected, it revealed a significant age effect with 6-year-olds scoring higher on the mentalistic level of TEC 1 (M = 2.25, SD = 0.64) than 4-year-olds (M = 1.45, SD = 1.01), F(1, 62) = 12.65, p = 0.001, η2 = 0.18. Finally, a 2 (Children's age: 4, 6) × 2 (Children's gender: girl, boy) analysis of variance (ANOVA) conducted on the reflective level of TEC 1 as a dependent variable revealed no significant effect of gender. As expected, it revealed a significant age effect with 6-year-olds scoring higher on the reflective level of TEC 1 (M = 1.04, SD = 0.69) than 4-year-olds (M = 0.40, SD = 0.50), F(1, 62) = 9.02, p = 0.004, η2 = 0.13. There was also a significant Age × Gender interaction effect, F(1, 62) = 8.04, p = 0.006, η2 = 0.12. There was no difference between girls and boys at age 4, F < 1, whereas girls (M = 1.46, SD = 0.52) outperformed boys (M = 0.67, SD = 0.62) at age 6, F(1, 27) = 13.36, p = 0.001, η2 = 0.34.

Similarly, a 2 (Children's age: 4, 6) × 2 (Children's gender: boy, girl) ANOVA conducted on the second administration of the TEC as a dependent variable revealed no significant effect of gender. There was, however, a significant age effect with 6-year-old children scoring higher on the TEC (M = 6.35, SD = 1.40) than 4-year-old children (M = 4.68, SD = 1.72), F(1, 62) = 16.56, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.21. Scores for TEC 1 and TEC 2 were significantly correlated, r(61) = 0.78, p = 0.01. A repeated measures ANOVA revealed that across both age groups, children scored higher on TEC 2 than on TEC 1, F(1, 62) = 22.88, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.27.

Relations between Parents' Emotion Talk, and Children's Emotion Understanding

Before conducting analyses, all data were screened. Children's TEC scores and mothers' emotion labels and explanation in each task were found to be normally distributed with kurtosis and skewness below 3.00 (Tabachnik and Fidell, 2007). In contrast, fathers' use of explanations during the storytelling task and explanations and labels during the reminiscence task were found to violate skewness and kurtosis assumptions. Removal of four outliers did not improve skewness and kurtosis to below 3.00. Thus, a square root transformation was applied to these variables in SPSS.

To examine which elements of mothers' and fathers' emotion talk were related to the TEC 1 and TEC 2, correlations were conducted between TEC 1 and TEC 2 and mothers' emotion talk (emotion labels and emotion explanations during the play-related storytelling task and the reminiscence task) and fathers' emotion talk (emotion labels and emotion explanations during the play-related storytelling task and the reminiscence task). Figure 1 indicates the relation between mothers' proportion of labels during the storytelling task and TEC 2 scores. These correlations were conducted separately for each group and combined across age groups using the transformed scores for three of the fathers' talk variables. Table 1 indicates that TEC1 and TEC 2 were highly correlated, r(61) = 0.78, p = 0.01, across both age groups combined. There was a significant relation between mothers' labels during the reminiscence task and TEC 2, r(61) = 0.28, p = 0.05. There was also a significant relation between fathers' labels during the reminiscence task (transformed variable) and TEC 2, r(61) = 0.28, p = 0.05, and fathers' explanations (transformed variable) and labels during the reminiscence task, r(61) = 0.27, p = 0.05. Fathers' use of labels during the two tasks was correlated, r(61) = 0.37, p = 0.003. There was a significant correlation between mothers' explanations and labels during the storytelling task, r(61) = 0.27, p = 0.05. Mothers' use of labels during both tasks was correlated, r(61) = 0.38, p < 0.01. Table 2 displays the correlations conducted separately for 4-year-old children. In the case of 4-year-old children TEC 1 and TEC 2 were related, r(61) = 0.73, p = 0.01. Table 3 displays the correlations conducted separately for 4-year-old children. There was a significant relation between 6-year-olds' TEC1 and TEC 2, r(61) = 0.65, p = 0.01. No correlations between parents' emotion talk and children's emotion understanding were significant when the statistics were conducted separately by age group, however as the tables indicate, there were relations between parents' talk variables. For example, when children were 4-years-old, there was a significant relation between mothers' use of labels during the two tasks, r(61) = 0.38, p < 0.05. For fathers, fathers' use of labels during the two tasks was significantly related, r(61) = 0.37, p < 0.01. When children were 6-years-old, there was also a relation between mothers' use of labels across the two tasks, r(61) = 0.43, p < 0.05.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Proportion of labels used by mothers during the storytelling task and TEC 2 scores.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Relations between parental talk and children's emotion understanding.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Relations between parental emotion talk, and children's emotion understanding at age 4.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Relations between parental emotion talk and children's emotion understanding at age 6.

Regression analyses

To examine whether scores on TEC 2 were predicted by mothers' and fathers' emotion talk after controlling for prior TEC scores, two hierarchical multiple regression analyses were conducted separately for each parent with TEC 2 as the dependent variable. Regressions were conducted separately because of inadequate statistical power (Field, 2005). In step 1, TEC 1 was entered. In step 2, the parent's proportion of labels and explanations during the play-related storytelling task and the reminiscence task were entered. As expected, TEC 1 predicted TEC 2. Above and beyond prior emotion understanding, fathers' talk did not predict children's emotion understanding. In contrast, mothers' emotion labels during the play-related storytelling task predicted children's understanding of emotions. Tables 4, 5 display the beta weights and standard errors.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Regression model predicting children's emotion understanding from maternal talk.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Regression model predicting children's emotion understanding from paternal talk.

Discussion

This study explored the relation between Spanish parent-child emotion talk and children's emotion understanding across two types of family narratives: a play-related storytelling task and a reminiscence task. Findings of this study support and extend the existing research on parent-child emotion talk and children's emotion understanding. The correlations between mothers' and fathers' emotion talk and children's emotion understanding were similar. Mothers' use of emotion labels during the play-related storytelling task predicted children's emotion understanding above and beyond prior emotion understanding, whereas, fathers' emotion talk did not predict children's emotion understanding above and beyond prior emotion understanding. Thus, after controlling for prior emotion understanding, mothers' influence continues to be more predictive of children's understanding than is fathers' influence. These findings will be discussed in greater detail below.

Mothers' use of labels during the reminiscence task and fathers' use of labels during the play-related storytelling task was related to children's emotion understanding at time 2. However, after controlling for prior emotion understanding, fathers' emotion talk did not predict children's emotion understanding. This finding is not consistent with Denham et al. (2010) who concluded that fathers' emotion talk is related to children's emotion understanding. There are a few reasons that could explain the different findings between both studies. We conjecture that Spanish mothers' and fathers' influence on their children's development of emotion understanding is more distinct to that of US mothers and fathers. Indeed, evidence suggests that mothers are the emotional gatekeepers of the family whereas fathers occupy playmate and disciplinarian roles (Garside and Klimes-Dougan, 2002; Lewis and Lamb, 2003; Bretherton et al., 2005; Denham et al., 2010). Perhaps fathers socialize children's emotions through rough and tumble play or through the way in which they discipline their children. Indeed, fathers' play styles predict children's later socio-emotional development (Carson and Parke, 1996). Alternatively, fathers may have been following children's lead from lack of daily experience with them, and used more emotion words when their children afforded such talk. Another possibility is that we cannot study mothers' and fathers' socialization of emotions using the same methodology and instead, should develop a specific method to examine fathers' contribution to children's emotion understanding.

One final explanation is that fathers tend to spend less time with children than do mothers (Craig, 2006). In Spain, this gender difference is exacerbated with mothers as children's main careers (Instituto de la Mujer, 2012). Less than 20% of the mothers worked fulltime outside the home in this sample with many stay-at-home mothers. In contrast, the fathers tended to work long hours and many reported working more than 50 h a week. Thus, how much time fathers typically spent with children is unknown and may be one of the reasons why their influence was smaller.

Second, mothers' use of emotion labels during the play-related storytelling predicted children's emotion understanding. We conjecture that when mothers labeled emotions during the play-related storytelling task, they drew children's attention to them and implicitly communicated to children that expressing emotions is acceptable. Indeed, Denham et al. (2010) suggest that mothers serve as emotional gatekeepers of the family. For this reason, we find it striking that mothers' emotion talk during reminiscing did not predict children's emotion understanding. Perhaps during reminiscing the emotional experience is too strong for children to learn from the experience compared to the storytelling task. Note that the TEC is a cognitive measure of emotion understanding rather than an expressive measure. Moreover, like a storytelling task, the children need to understand emotions another person experiences to answer questions correctly on the TEC, which may help to explain why there is a relation between mothers' labels during the storytelling task and the TEC, but not during the reminiscence task. There may be other elements of mothers' emotion talk while reminiscing that predict children's emotion understanding, such as mothers' elaborateness or the discussion of causes and consequences of emotions.

Little research on parent-child emotion talk and its relations with children's emotion understanding has been conducted outside English-speaking countries or with fathers. This study demonstrates that we cannot simply extend the literature on mothers to fathers. Fathers have a distinct influence on children's development that must be investigated. Research needs to include fathers in more cross-cultural research to examine how their participation influences children.

Limitations and Directions for Future Research

The present study found some small relations between parents' emotion talk and children's emotion understanding; however, many of the correlations were not significant. Thus, we need to be cautious when interpreting the findings of this study. Moreover, when the correlations were conducted separately by age group, there were no significant relations between parental talk and children's emotion understanding. The lack of findings may have resulted from the small sample size when the ages were split or resulted from other factors influencing children's emotion understanding. Future research should examine other factors such as children's language abilities, given that language ability influence children's emotion understanding (Harris et al., 2005; Grazzani-Gavazzi and Ornaghi, 2011). Due to time restrictions, children did not complete a language ability task. Second, participants from the present study were Spanish from the same socioeconomic status. Thus, we have to be cautious when generalizing findings. Future research should examine the relation between parents' emotion talk and children's emotion understanding across different cultures and socioeconomic statuses. Third, the emotion task was a snapshot of parent-child interactions and focused only on verbal interactions. As mentioned previously, analysis of non-verbal cues may have demonstrated relations between fathers' behaviors and children's understanding.

Conclusion

In conclusion, findings from the present study add to the existing knowledge of the relation between parents' emotion talk and children's emotion understanding. Findings indicated that whereas mothers' use of emotion labels during the play-related storytelling task predicted children's emotion understanding, fathers' emotion talk in this task did not predict children's emotion understanding after controlling for previous emotion understanding. These findings suggest that mothers and fathers may have different influences on children.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This project was completed as part of Ana Aznar's doctoral dissertation at Kingston University. The authors would like to thank Drs. Frederic Vallee-Tourangeau and Jess Prior for being members of her committee and Drs. Patrick Leman, Robin Banerjee, and Claudia Uller for their helpful comments as external examiners. Finally, they would like to thank all the families who participated in this research.

References

Bamberg, M., and Damrad-Frye, R. (1991). On the ability to provide evaluative comments: further explorations of children's narrative competencies. J. Child Lang. 18, 689–710. doi: 10.1017/S0305000900011314

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bloom, L., and Capatides, J. B. (1987). Expression of affect and the emergence of language. Child Dev. 58, 1513–1522. doi: 10.2307/1130691

CrossRef Full Text

Bretherton, I., Lambert, J. D., and Golby, B. (2005). Involved fathers of preschool children as seen by themselves and their wives: accounts of attachment, socialization, and companionship. Attach. Hum. Dev. 7, 229–251. doi: 10.1080/14616730500138341

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bretherton, I., Ridgeway, D., and Cassidy, J. (1990). “Assessing internal working models of the attachment relationship,” in Attachment in the Preschool Years: Theory, Research, and Intervention, eds M. T. Greenberg, D. Cicchetti, and E. M. Cummings (Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press), 273–308.

Brody, L. (1999). Gender, Emotion and the Family. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Brownell, C. A., Svetlova, M., Anderson, R., Nichols, S. R., and Drummond, J. (2013). Socialization of early prosocial behaviour: parents' talk about emotions is associated with sharing and helping in toddlers. Infancy 18, 91–119. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-7078.2012.00125.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Bruner, J. S. (1990). Acts of Meaning. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.

Carson, J. L., and Parke, R. D. (1996). Reciprocal negative affect in parent-child interactions and children's peer competence. Child Dev. 67, 2217–2226. doi: 10.2307/1131619

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cassidy, J., Parke, R. D., Butkovsky, L., and Braungart, J. M. (1992). Family-peer connections: the roles of emotional expressiveness within the family and children's understanding of emotion. Child Dev. 63, 603–618. doi: 10.2307/1131349

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cervantes, C. A. (2002). Explanatory emotion talk in Mexican immigrant and Mexican American families. Hisp. J. Behav. Sci. 24, 138–163. doi: 10.1177/0739986302024002003

CrossRef Full Text

Cervantes, C. A., and Callanan, M. A. (1998). Labels and explanations in mother-child emotion talk: age and gender differentiation. Dev. Psychol. 34, 88–98. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.34.1.88

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Cicchetti, D., Ackerman, B. P., and Izard, C. E. (1995). Emotions and emotion regulation in developmental psychopathology. Dev. Psychopathol. 7, 1–10. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400006301

CrossRef Full Text

Craig, L. (2006). Does father care mean fathers share? A comparison of how mothers and fathers in intact families spend time with children. Gend. Soc. 20, 259–281. doi: 10.1177/0891243205285212

CrossRef Full Text

Curenton, S. M., and Craig, M. J. (2011). Shared-reading versus oral storytelling: associations with preschoolers' prosocial skills and problem behaviours. Early Child Dev. Care 181, 123–146. doi: 10.1080/03004430903292208

CrossRef Full Text

Cutting, A. L., and Dunn, J. (1999). Theory of mind, emotion understanding, language, and family background: individual differences and interrelations. Child Psychol. 70, 853–865. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00061

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

De Rosnay, M., and Hughes, C. (2006). Conversation and theory of mind: do children talk their way to socio-cognitive understanding? Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 24, 7–37. doi: 10.1348/026151005X82901

CrossRef Full Text

Denham, S. A. (1997). “When I have a bad dream, mommy holds me”: preschoolers' conceptions of emotions, parental socialization, and emotional competence. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 20, 301–319. doi: 10.1080/016502597385351

CrossRef Full Text

Denham, S. A., and Auerbach, S. (1995). Mother-child dialogue about emotions and preschoolers' emotional competence. Genet. Soc. Gen. Psychol. Monogr. 121, 313–337.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Denham, S. A., Bassett, H. H., Hamada, A., and Wyatt, T. (2010). Gender differences in the socialization of preschoolers' emotional competence. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 2010, 29–49. doi: 10.1002/cd.267

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Denham, S. A., Bassett, H. H., and Wyatt, T. (2007). “The socialization of emotional competence,” in Handbook of Socialization: Theory and Research, eds J. E. Grusec and P. D. Hastings (New York, NY: Guildford Press), 614–637.

Denham, S. A., McKinley, M., Couchoud, E. A., and Holt, R. (1990). Emotional and behavioural predictors of preschool peer ratings. Child Dev. 61, 1145–1152. doi: 10.2307/1130882

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Denham, S. A., Renwick-DeBardi, S., and Hewes, S. (1994). Emotional communication between mothers and preschoolers: relations with emotional competence. Merrill. Palmer. Q. 40, 488–508.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

Denham, S. A., Workman, E., Cole, P., Weissbrod, C., Kendziora, K., and Zahn-Waxler, C. (2000). Parental contributions to externalizing and internalizing patterns in young children at risk for conduct disorder. Dev. Psychopathol. 12, 23–45. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400001024

CrossRef Full Text

Dibiase, R., and Gunnoe, J. (2004).Gender and culture differences in touching behaviour. J. Soc. Psychol. 144, 49–62. doi: 10.3200/SOCP.144.1.49-62

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Dunn, J., Bretherton, I., and Munn, P. (1987). Conversations about feeling states between mothers and their young children. Dev. Psychol. 23, 132–139. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.23.1.132

CrossRef Full Text

Dunn, J., Brown, J., and Beardsall, L. (1991). Family talk about feeling states and children's later understanding of other's emotions. Dev. Psychol. 27, 448–455. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.27.3.448

CrossRef Full Text

Dunsmore, J. C., Her, P., Halberstadt, A. G., and Perez-Rivera, M. B. (2009). Parents' beliefs about emotions and children's recognition of parents' emotions. J. Nonverbal Behav. 33, 121–140. doi: 10.1007/s10919-008-0066-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Field, A. (2005). Discovering Statistics using SPSS. London: SAGE.

Fivush, R. (1989). Exploring sex differences in the emotional content of mother-child conversations about the past. Sex Roles 20, 675–691. doi: 10.1007/BF00288079

CrossRef Full Text

Fivush, R. (1993). “Developmental perspectives on autobiographical recall,” in Child Victims, Child Witnesses: Understanding and Improving Testimony, eds G. S. Goodman and B. L. Bottoms (New York, NY: Guildford), 1–24.

Fivush, R. (1998). Methodological challenges in the study of emotional socialization. Psychol. Inq. 9, 281–283. doi: 10.1207/s15327965pli0904_5

CrossRef Full Text

Fivush, R., Brotman, M. A., Buckner, J. P., and Goodman, S. H. (2000). Gender differences in parent-child emotion narratives. Sex Roles 42, 233–252. doi: 10.1023/A:1007091207068

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fivush, R., Marin, K., McWilliams, K., and Bohanek, J. G. (2009). Family reminiscing style: parent gender and emotional focus in relation to child well-being. J. Cogn. Dev. 10, 210–235. doi: 10.1080/15248370903155866

CrossRef Full Text

Flavell, J. H., Green, F. L., and Flavell, E. R. (1993). Children's understanding of the stream of consciousness. Child Dev. 64, 387–398. doi: 10.2307/1131257

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Fletcher, A. C., Darling, N. E., Steinberg, L., and Dornbusch, S. (1995). The company they keep: relation of adolescents' adjustment and behaviour to their friends' perceptions of authoritative parenting in the social network. Dev. Psychol. 31, 300–310. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.31.2.300

CrossRef Full Text

Garner, P. W. (2003). Child and family correlates of toddlers' emotional and behavioral responses to a mishap. Infant Ment. Health J. 24, 580–596. doi: 10.1002/imhj.10076

CrossRef Full Text

Garner, P. W. (2010). Emotional competence and its influences on teaching and learning. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 22, 297–321. doi: 10.1007/s10648-010-9129-4

CrossRef Full Text

Garner, P. W., Dunsmore, J. C., and Southam-Gerrow, M. (2008). Mother-child conversations about emotions: linkages to child aggression and prosocial behaviour. Soc. Dev. 17, 259–277. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00424.x

CrossRef Full Text

Garner, P. W., Jones, D. C., and Miner, J. L. (1994). Social competence among low-income preschoolers: emotion socialization practices and social cognitive correlates. Child Dev. 65, 622–637. doi: 10.2307/1131405

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Garner, P. W., and Power, T. G. (1996). Preschoolers' emotional control in the disappointment paradigm and its relation to temperament, emotional knowledge, and family expressiveness. Child Dev. 67, 1406–1419. doi: 10.2307/1131708

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Garrett-Peters, P., Mills-Koonce, R., Zerwas, S., Cox, M., and Vernon-Feagans, L. (2011). Fathers' early emotion talk: associations with income, ethnicity, and family factors. J. Marriage Fam. 73, 335–353. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2010.00810.x

CrossRef Full Text

Garside, R. B., and Klimes-Dougan, B. (2002). Socialization of discrete negative emotions: gender differences and links with psychological distress. Sex Roles 47, 115–128. doi: 10.1023/A:1021090904785

CrossRef Full Text

Grazzani-Gavazzi, I., and Ornaghi, V. (2011). Emotional state talk and emotion understanding” A training study with preschool children. J. Child Lang. 38, 1124–1139. doi: 10.1017/S0305000910000772

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Halberstadt, A. G., Crisp, V. W., and Eaton, K. L. (1999). “Family expressiveness: a retrospective and new directions for research,” in The Social Context of Nonverbal Behaviour. Studies in Emotion and Social Interaction, eds P. Phillippot, R. S. Feldman, and E. Coats (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 109–115.

Halberstadt, A. G., and Lozada, F. T. (2011). Emotion development in infancy through the lens of culture. Emot. Rev. 3, 158–168. doi: 10.1177/1754073910387946

CrossRef Full Text

Harris, P. L., de Rosnay, M., and Pons, F. (2005). Language and children's understanding of mental states. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 14, 69–73. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00337.x

CrossRef Full Text

Harris, P. L., Donnelly, K., Guz, G. R., and Pitt-Watson, R. (1986). Children's understanding of the distinction between real and apparent emotion. Child Dev. 57, 895–909. doi: 10.2307/1130366

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Harris, P. L., Johnson, C., Hutton, D., Andrews, G., and Cooke, T. (1989). Young children's theory of mind and emotion. Cogn. Emot. 3, 379–400. doi: 10.1080/02699938908412713

CrossRef Full Text

Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1991). Mother-child conversation in different social classes and communicative settings. Child Dev. 62, 782–796. doi: 10.2307/1131177

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Hoffman, M. L. (2000). Empathy and Moral Development: Implications for Caring and Justice. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Instituto de la Mujer. Retrieved October 13, 2012, Available online at: http://www.inmujer.es/

Izard, C., Fine, S., Schultz, D., Mostow, A., Ackerman, B., and Youngstrom, E. (2001). Emotion knowledge as a predictor of social behaviour and academic competence in children at risk. Psychol. Sci. 12, 18–23. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00304

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Jimenez, M., Alonso, M. I., Garcia, G., Marco, B., and Gimenez-Dasi, M. (2013). “Mejorar las competencias socioemocionales en ninos gitanos: una intervencion a traves de la filosofia para ninos,” in Poster presented at the Jornadas de Educacion Emocional (Barcelona).

Joshi, M. S., and MacLean, M. (1994). Indian and English children's understanding of the distinction between real and apparent emotion. Child Dev. 65, 1372–1384. doi: 10.2307/1131504

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kim, J., and Cicchetti, D. (2010). Longitudinal pathways linking child maltreatment, emotion regulation, peer relations, and psychopathology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 51, 706–716. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02202.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Kring, A. M., and Bachorowski, J. A. (1999). Emotions and psychopathology. Cogn. Emot. 13, 575–599. doi: 10.1080/026999399379195

CrossRef Full Text

Kuebli, J., and Fivush, R. (1992). Gender differences in parent-child conversations about past emotions. Sex Roles 27, 683–698. doi: 10.1007/BF02651097

CrossRef Full Text

Laible, D. J. (2004). Mother-child discourse surrounding a child's past behaviour at 30 months: links to emotional understanding and early conscience development at 36 months. Merrill. Palmer. Q. 50, 159–180. doi: 10.1353/mpq.2004.0013

CrossRef Full Text

Laible, D., and Song, J. (2006). Constructing emotional and relational understanding: the role of affect and mother-child discourse. Merrill. Palmer. Q. 52, 44–69. doi: 10.1353/mpq.2006.0006

CrossRef Full Text

Lamb, M. E. (Ed.). (2010). “How do fathers influence child development? Let me count the ways,” in The Role of the Father in Child Development (Hoboken, NJ: Wiley & Sons).

Lewis, C., and Lamb, M. E. (2003). Fathers' influences on children's development: the evidence from two-parent families. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 18, 211–228. doi: 10.1007/BF03173485

CrossRef Full Text

Martin, R. M., and Green, J. A. (2005). The use of emotion explanations by mothers: relation to preschoolers' gender and understanding of emotions. Soc. Dev. 14, 229–249. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2005.00300.x

CrossRef Full Text

Oppenheim, D., Emde, R., and Winfrey, N. (1993). “Parent-child constructions of emotion narratives: associations with children's narratives and developmental course,” in Paper Presented at the Biennial Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development (New Orleans, LA).

Pancsofar, N., and Vernon-Feagans, L. (2006). Mothers and fathers language input to young children: contributions to later language development. J. Appl. Dev. Psychol. 27, 571–587. doi: 10.1016/j.appdev.2006.08.003

CrossRef Full Text

Perez-Rivera, M. B., and Dunsmore, J. (2011). Mothers' acculturation and beliefs about emotions, mother-child emotion discourse, and children's emotion understanding in Latino families. Early Educ. Dev. 22, 324–354. doi: 10.1080/10409281003702000

CrossRef Full Text

Pons, F., and Harris, P. L. (2005). Longitudinal change and longitudinal stability of individual differences in children's emotion understanding. Cogn. Emot. 19, 1158–1174. doi: 10.1080/02699930500282108

CrossRef Full Text

Pons, F., Harris, P. L., and de Rosnay, M. (2004). Emotion comprehension between 3 and 11 years: developmental periods and hierarchical organizations. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol. 1, 127–152. doi: 10.1080/17405620344000022

CrossRef Full Text

Pons, F., Harris, P. L., and Doudin, P. A. (2002). Teaching emotion understanding. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 7, 293–304. doi: 10.1007/BF03173538

CrossRef Full Text

Pons, F., Lawson, J., Harris, P. L., and de Rosnay, M. (2003). Individual differences in children's emotion understanding: effects of age and language.Scand. J. Psychol. 44, 347–353. doi: 10.1111/1467-9450.00354

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Reese, E., Haden, C. A., and Fivush, R. (1996). Mothers, fathers. Daughters, sons: gender differences in autobiographical reminiscing. Res. Lang. Soc. Interact. 29, 27–56. doi: 10.1207/s15327973rlsi2901_3

CrossRef Full Text

Rydell, A. M., Berlin, L., and Bohlin, G. (2003). Emotionality, emotion regulation, and adaptation among 5-to 8-year-old children. Emotion 3, 30–47. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.3.1.30

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Sales, J. M., Fivush, R., and Peterson, C. (2003). Parental reminiscing about positive and negative events. J. Cogn. Dev. 4, 185–209. doi: 10.1207/S15327647JCD0402_03

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Suveg, C., Shaffer, A., Morelen, D., and Thomassin, K. (2011). Links between maternal and child psychopathology symptoms: mediation through child emotion regulation and moderation through maternal behavior. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 42, 507–520. doi: 10.1007/s10578-011-0223-8

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Tabachnik, B. G., and Fidell, L. S. (2007). Experimental Designs using ANOVA. Belmont, CA: Thomson/Brooks/Cole.

Valiente, C., Swanson, J., and Eisenberg, N. (2012). Linking students' emotions and academic achievement: when and why emotions matter. Child Dev. Perspect. 6, 129–135. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00192.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wang, Q. (2001). Culture effects on adults' earliest childhood recollection and self –description: implications for the relation between memory and the self. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 81, 220–233. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.220

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., and Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. Child Dev. 72, 655–684. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00304

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Wellman, H. M., and Lagattuta, K. M. (2004). Theory of mind for learning and teaching: the nature of role and explanation. Cogn. Dev. 19, 479–497. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2004.09.003

CrossRef Full Text

Zahn-Waxler, C. (2010). Socialization of emotion: who influences whom and how? New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 2010, 101–109. doi: 10.1002/cd.271

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: parent-child emotion talk, children's understanding of emotions, emotion understanding, culture, children's socialization of emotions

Citation: Aznar A and Tenenbaum HR (2013) Spanish parents' emotion talk and their children's understanding of emotion. Front. Psychol. 4:670. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00670

Received: 01 July 2013; Paper pending published: 01 August 2013;
Accepted: 06 September 2013; Published online: 24 September 2013.

Edited by:

Francesc Sidera, University of Girona, Spain

Reviewed by:

Jennifer Van Reet, Providence College, USA
Francesc Sidera, University of Girona, Spain
Ilaria Grazzani, University of Milano-Bicocca, Italy

Copyright © 2013 Aznar and Tenenbaum. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ana Aznar, Faculty of Arts and Social Sciences, Kingston University, Penrhyn Road, Kingston Upon Thames, London KT1 2EE, UK e-mail: anaaznar81@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.